A Sentence Made by Men:
Muted Group Theory Revisited
The European Journal of Women's Studies, Vol.6, 1999: 21-29
Celia J. Wall
MURRAY STATE UNIVERSITY, USA
Pat Gannon-Leary
UNIVERSITY OF NORTHUMBRIA AT NEWCASTLE
Before a woman can write exactly as she wishes to
write, she has many difficulties to face. To begin with, there is the technical
difficulty - so simple, apparently, in reality, so baffling - that the very
form of the sentence does not fit her. It is a sentence made by men. (Woolf,
1979: 48)
Virginia
Woolf gives insight into the problems inherent in using a medium which, over
the centuries, has been dominated by men. For some feminists there is no such
thing as neutral language: 'the entire system, since it belongs to and is
controlled by men, is permeated by sexism through and through' (Cameron, 1985:
91). The somewhat simplistic belief that it is through language that reality is
constructed accounts for the focus of much feminist literature on the
'silencing' of women through the control of language. Cameron expresses the
concern, 'that language, or the lack of an authentic (non-male) language,
profoundly affects women's ability to understand and change their situation'
(Cameron, 1985: 92). Muted group theory, developed by anthropologists Shirley and
Edwin Ardener in the late 1960s, has received a great deal of attention from
feminists concerned with the nature of language.
While in the 1970s and 1980s, feminist
studies established gender as a legitimate mode of historical explanation,
their approach laid emphasis on the ideology of the public (male) and private
(female) domains. In the 1990s feminist research has criticised this concept as
overly schematic and somewhat reductionist, challenging the earlier
'monotheistic' fixation to gender as the explanatory variable. Does the view of
separate spheres as an outmoded paradigm negate the Ardeners' theory?
The Ardeners contended that there were
'dominant modes of expression in any society which have been generated by the
dominant structure within it' (E. Ardener, 1975b: 20). To be heard and heeded
an individual must use this dominant mode of expression. The use of an
Alternative, 'individual' mode of expression will not be heard. To be
understood, the would-be communicator must suppress her own mode of expression
in favour of the dominant mode and thus she is 'muted'. Muted does not
necessarily mean silent: 'the important issue is whether they are able to say
all they would wish to say, where and when they wish to say it' (E. Ardener,
1975b: 21). Generally, in a situation where gender is a consideration, women
are therefore the muted group.
Muted group theory was originally
introduced by Edwin Ardener in a preface to his study on rituals of the Bakweri
women of Cameroon (first published in LaFontaine [1972], extended in E. Ardener
[1975a]). It was intended to explain the lack of anthropological data then
available on women and 'to encourage anthropologists and others to pay more
attention than they did at the time to spheres of communication and modes of
expression' (E. Ardener, 1975b: 20) of commonly overlooked groups. Ardener
noted that women as 'a human group that forms about half of any population and
is even in a majority at certain ages' (E. Ardener, 1975a: 1) were neglected in
social anthropological studies.
The methods of social anthropology . . . of the last
forty years have purported to 'crack the code' of a vast range of societies,
without any direct reference to the female group. At the level of 'observation'
in fieldwork, the behaviour of women has, of course, like that of men, been
exhaustively plotted. . . When we come to that second or 'meta' level of
fieldwork, the vast body of debate, discussion, question and answer, that
social anthropologists really depend upon to give conviction to their
interpretations, there is a real imbalance. (F. Ardener, 1975a: 1)
Even
women anthropologists, 'of whom so much was hoped' (E. Ardener, 1975a: 1), had
failed to redress the balance.
In
analysing this situation, Ardener identified two parts to the problem. First,
what he termed the technical part, was that females reportedly were more
difficult to access and interview: 'Ethnographers report that women cannot be
reached so easily as men: they giggle when young, snort when old, reject the
question, laugh at the topic and the like' (E. Ardener, 1975a: 2). Ardener
contended that ethnographers, male and female, were more inclined to see and
interpret the model of a society in the same terms as the males being
interviewed, consequently identifying more with the males and their views.
Males would, therefore, be viewed as more articulate than the females.
Second, Ardener identified the analytical
part of the problem.
If the models of a society made by most ethnographers
tend to be models derived from the male portion of that society, how does the
symbolic weight of that other mass of persons - half or more of a normal human
population, as we have accepted - express itself? (E. Ardener, 1975a: 3)
Ardener pointed out that it was not unusual
then for ethnographers to return from a study of a particular society having
talked only to men. Despite the exclusion of the women, professional criticism
was unlikely because, Ardener concluded, 'models of society that women can
provide are not of the kind acceptable at first sight to men or to
ethnographers' (E. Ardener, 1975a: 3). The male model of society would be
considered by the dominant group (men) to be accurate and true since they would
see nothing lacking in the model presented. If the dominant group does not
complain or criticize, who will?
The
Ardeners' thesis is that women, due to their structural positions, have
different models of reality from the male-dominated societal model. Women's
models often take a non-verbal, inarticulate, veiled form in contrast to the male
discourse, which is more verbal and explicit, matching the usual discourse of
western social science. Although feminist critics of anthropology have accused
androcentric ethnographers of ignoring women in the societies studied, the
Ardeners' work exposes a more complex problem, claiming as it does that the
fact women rarely 'speak' in social anthropological reports is due to the
demands made by both male and female social scientists for articulate models.
This problem was compounded because Ardener believed that 'they [women] lack
the metalanguage for its discussion' (E. Ardener, 1975a: 3), and are thus
unable to share their perception of society with ethnographers. Ardener blamed
this inability to articulate on the male-dominated structure of society. Henley
and Kramarae's (1994) discussion of cross-sex miscommunication similarly
indicated an inequality in the metastructure of interpretation resulting in the
accepted interpretation of an interaction being that of the more powerful
person, usually the male. Men were the 'exploiting' or 'dominate' class and
women the 'exploited' or 'suppressed' - in other words, the 'muted' - class.
What caused a group to be 'muted'? Ardener explained, 'it is muted simply
because it does not form part of the dominate communication system of the
society, expressed as it must be through the dominate ideology' (E. Ardener,
1975b: 22). Ardener refers to muted not as 'dumb' but as being 'of a reduced
level of perceptibility' (E. Ardener, 1975b: 22).
In
Defining Females, Shirley Ardener
provided evidence from various studies to support the muted group theory. One
such study, by Carole Humphrey, detailed the complicated speech requirement of
Mongolian daughters-in-law: 'Instead of thinking of a name, then saying it,
these women must think of a name, then think of a rule-bounded substitute, then
say that' (S. Ardener, 1978: 22). Another example of indirect speech was
provided by M. Wolff 'It seems that in China women commonly speak
"through" their young male charges and sons, attributing their own
views and needs to them' (S. Ardener, 1978: 22). Ardener concluded that the
'requirement for some categories of women to be "tongue-tied" is well
illustrated' (S. Ardener, 1978: 23) by such studies.
The muted group theory has been influential
in the feminist movement, being drawn upon and modified by several theorists,
especially Dale Spender and Cheris Kramarae. Spender has expanded on the
Ardeners' model to support her own beliefs on how men control language.
According to Spender, men, through their control over meaning, are able to
impose their own view of the world on everyone. Women's voices trying to
express women's experiences are rarely heard because they must be expressed in
a language system not designed for their interests and concerns. Unable to
symbolise their experience in the male language, women take one of two routes:
one path requires internalising male reality - alienation; the other is being
unable to speak at all - silence: 'The talkativeness of women has been gauged
in comparison not with men but with silence.. . . When silence is the desired
state for women. . . then any talk in which a woman engages can be too much'
(Spender, 1980: 42). Silence is deplored in feminist writings because it
symbolises passivity and powerlessness. The muted group and female deficit
theories employ descriptors implying inadequacy: women's communication is
variously described as handicapped, maladaptive, in need of remediation,
deviant (from the masculine norm or, more accurately, the male cultural form),
silenced, inarticulate and inferior. The titles of the works of Spender and
others illustrate the concern with silence as an obstacle to self-expression.
Silence and 'mutedness' do not merely refer to 'inability or reluctance to
create utterances in conversational exchange' but also denote 'failure to
produce a separate, socially significant discourse' (Gal, 1994: 408). Unable to
express their structurally generated views in the dominant masculine discourse,
women are neither understood nor heeded, becoming inarticulate, 'muted' or
silent. Even if they talk a lot they may not express their own, different,
social reality.
Kramarae
(1981) discusses several hypotheses suggested by muted theory. First she
advances the hypothesis that, while females are more likely to have difficulty
expressing themselves fluently within dominant (public) modes of expression,
males have more difficulty in understanding what members of the other gender
mean. She posits that, in both verbal conventions and non-verbal behaviour,
females are liable to find ways to express themselves outside the dominant
public modes of expression used by males. A further contention is the
likelihood that females will state dissatisfaction with the dominant public
modes of expression. Those women refusing to live by the ideas of social
organisations held by the dominant group will, Kramarae suggests, change
dominant public modes of expression as they consciously and verbally reject
those ideas. She further suggests that females are less likely to coin words
that become widely recognised and used by both men and women. Finally she
advances the hypothesis that females' sense of humour differs from that of
males.
Kramarae draws upon her own previous
studies and research done by other scholars for evidence to support or refute
hypotheses. For example, in examining the question of the relative verbal
skills of women and men, she states, 'that females will have more difficulty
than males expressing themselves seems implicit in the muted group theory',
however, she adds, 'most of the information available on sex differences and
verbal skills does not seem to support such an hypothesis' (Kramarae, 1981: 5).
Kramarae's balanced look at the tenets of muted group theory indicates areas of
potential weakness. Evidence advanced by the Ardeners in support of their
theory seems narrow in scope and relevance. Is the fact that a Chinese woman
must 'speak' through a male relative really relevant anywhere but China? The
same could be asked about the Mongolian daughters-in-law's complicated speech
requirement. How does that relate to females elsewhere? The Ardeners have taken
examples of isolated situations and generalised their relevance to a larger
world.
The world has changed a great deal in the
past two and a half decades since the Ardeners first introduced their theory
Women have found their voices and are speaking out more and more even if it is
in the 'male mode of expression'. In ethnographic contexts, Gal cites an elite
intellectual study group, the Men's and Women's Club of London in the 1880s. In
discussions of sexuality, although the club rules asserted gender equality,
rule 17 - accepted by all members - stipulated that discussion must be within a
Darwinian framework, a proviso which 'both assured and hid men's dominance'
according to Gal (1994: 421) since 'women members lacked such scientific
knowledge'. Kramarae's discussion of the muted group theory and how men have
appropriated the important public use of language, leaving women with the
'trivial' private language domain, includes an account of how,
. . . some of the women in the early
slavery abolition movement in the United States were encouraged to use in their
writing rhetorical principles set up entirely by male British and American
orators, and to sit silent on the platform while male ministers and relatives
read the women's public address. (Kramarae, 1981: 29)
Puckett asserts that the muted group model
'is problematic because it fails to acknowledge the empirical fact that women
do in fact speak, and in public. A brief perusal of popular and academic
culture attests to this' (Puckett, 1986: 7). Cameron believes that:
What the model really fails to show is that muted
groups lack a language, and that dominant groups are able to appropriate all linguistic
resources. The alternative suggestion, that women communicate adequately with
each other but are institutionally constrained/negatively judged in the public
(male) arena, is much more plausible. (Cameron, 1985:107-8)
However,
as stated at the outset, the private/public (female/male) arena division has
recently been discredited, and acknowledgement made that separate spheres are
rarely truly separate.
Despite global changes and acknowledged
problems with the muted group theory, it still has relevance and its three
basic tenets on how a dominant group and a subordinate group communicate have
applicability beyond the gender based. First, women and men perceive the world
differently because of the different experiences afforded them by the division of
labour. Second, since men dominate politically, their mode of expression is
dominant. The corollary of this is that alternative (e.g. women's) modes of
expression are less acceptable. Finally, in order to express themselves and
participate in society, women must use the dominant mode of expression in
preference to their own, necessitating translation or becoming bilingual,
speaking both women's and men's language.
These basic tenets may be extended to other
groups and situations where a dominate entity and a subordinate one must
communicate each with the other. An example of the theory's relevance and use
may be found in an article by Carol Colfer, whose interest in the Ardeners'
theory 'was stimulated by the problem in international development of communication
between development personnel and members of the populations with whom they
work' (Colfer, 1983: 263). Colfer reports on three ethnographic examples of
situations in which 'unequals' had to communicate with one another. The result
in each case was 'inarticulateness'. The observed cases were diverse:
rural/urban dwellers in Iran; women/ men in a small American village; and
scientists (of both genders) from the soft/hard scientific disciplines. In fact
Colfer's examples were more supportive of 'inarticulateness' than those of the
Ardeners. In her conclusion Colfer comments on the fact that differential
power and status have a tendency to interfere with the free expression of ideas
from those of lower status. This inhibition of free expression reinforces the
integrated -but incomplete - world views held by the powerful, since it denies
them access not only to alternative perspectives but also to information which
will not fit neatly into their cognitive models. One implication of the
necessity for lower status peoples to adapt to the dominant models while
retaining their counterpart models is that, in order to function adequately in
life, greater cognitive complexity may be required of them than is required of
elites.
Colfer's
aim, in studying muted group theory, was to improve communication between
development personnel and the Third World groups with whom they worked. Having
established a theoretical framework for such communication using the muted
group theory, she noted that, 'An important responsibility. . . then, lies
squarely with the elites, to create a supportive environment which encourages
the expression of counterpart models. I see such improved communications as
critical to the success of development programs' (Colfer, 1983: 279).
One tenet of muted group theory ('to
express themselves and participate in society women must use the dominant mode
of expression') was tested in a study by Lituchy and Wiswall, although no
reference to muted group theory was made in the study, which aimed to determine
if the use of feminine speech pattern affected the 'credibility and
believability of women in organisations' (Lituchy and Wiswall, 1991: 453).
Research has shown that women's and men's speech patterns differ in word
choice, intonation and use of numerals with the consequent result that women
are viewed by males and females alike as being less assertive, more uncertain
and Jacking power. Women adopting male speech patterns are seen as more
assertive, self-confident and believable. The Lituchy and Wiswall study found
that
. . . decisions made by male and female
subordinates using masculine speech patterns were more likely to be accepted by
male listeners, whereas differences in speech patterns did not affect the
acceptance rate by female subjects. Male listeners also had more confidence in
subordinates (of either sex) with masculine speech patterns. (Lituchy and
Wiswall, 1991: 460)
The above studies provide evidence in
support of the muted group theory and exemplify its wider applicability both to
dominate/subordinate relationship, as a source of advice on how to improve
communication, and to organisations, offering insight into managers'
perceptions. Muted group theory can aid understanding in the communication
processes between females and males but, as the work of researchers such as
Henley and Kramarae (1994) in connection with the 'feminine deficit' theory has
highlighted, there is a need for a combination of disparate types of research.
Henley and Kramarae included social and anthropological studies as well as
psychological and linguistic ones in their discussion of male/female
miscominunication. If domination and resistance are matters of interactional
practice not just structure then the focus should extend beyond mutedness as a
structural product to the processes by which women are rendered mute.
Understanding the processes of dominance and muting requires a broader analysis
of the context - political, economic and institutional - in which reality is
negotiated. As communications studies researchers, the authors hope to initiate
a re-evaluation of muted group theory within communication theory and to
stimulate further research into - and understanding of - this concept.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Ardener, E. (1975a) 'Belief and the Problem
of Women', pp. 1-17 in S. Ardener (ed.) Perceiving
Women. London: Malaby Press.
Ardener, B. (1975b) 'The
"Problem" Revisited', pp. 19-27 in S. Ardener (ed.) Perceiving Women. London: Malaby Press.
Ardener, S. (ed.) (1975) Perceiving Women. London: Malaby Press.
Ardener, S. (1978) Defining Females: The Nature of Women in Society. London: Croom
Helm.
Buckley, S. (1997) Broken Silence: Voices of Japanese Feminism. Los Angeles:
University of California Press.
Burton, P., K.K. Dyson and S. Ardener
(1994) Bilingual Woman: Anthropological
Approaches to Second-Language Use. Oxford: Berg.
Cameron, D. (1985) Feminism and Linguistic Theory. London: Macmillan.
Coates, J. (1993) Women, Men and Language, 2nd
edn. London: Longman.
Colfer, C.J. (1983) 'On Communication among
"Unequals"', International
Journal of Intercultural Relations 7: 263~3.
Frank, F. and F. Anshen (1983) Language and the Sexes. Albany: State
University of New York Press.
Gal, 5. (1994) 'Between Speech and Silence: The
Problematics of Research on Language and Gender', pp. 407-31 in C. Roman, S.
Juhasz and C. Miller (eds) The Women and
Language Debate: A Sourcebook. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Henley, N.M. and C. Kramarae (1994)
'Gender, Power, and Miscommunication', pp. 38~06 in C. Roman, S. Juhasz and C. Miller
(eds) The Women and Language Debate: A
Sourcebook. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Kramarae, C~ (1981) Women and Men Speaking. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
LaFontaine, J.S. (1972) The Interpretation of Ritual. London:
Tavistock.
Lituchy, T.R. and W.J. Wiswall (1991) 'The
Role of Masculine and Feminine Speech Patterns in Proposal Acceptance', Management Communication Quarterly 4:
45~65.
Puckett, T.F. (1986) 'Toward Womanspeak: Explication
and Critique of Spender, Lacan, Irigaray and Kristeva’s Perspectives on Women
Speaking', paper presented at the annual meeting of the International
Communications Association, San Francisco, CA, ERIC Document Reproduction
Service No. ED 310433.
Roman, C., S. Juhasz and C. Miller (eds)
(1994) The Women and Language Debate: A
Sourcebook. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press
Spender, D. (1980) Man Made Language. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Tannen, D. (1995) Talking
from 9 to 5: How Women's and Men's Conversational Styles Affect Who Gets Heard,
Who Gets Credit, and What Gets Done at Work. London: Virago.
Woolf, V. (1979) Women and Writing. London: Women's Press. (Originally published
1929.)
Zimmerman, D.H. and C. West (1975) 'Sex
Roles, Interruptions and Silences in Conversation', pp. 10~29 in B. Thorne and
N. Henley (eds) Language and Sex:
Difference and Dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Celia J. Wall is an Associate Professor in the Department of Journalism and Mass
Communication at Murray State University in Murray, Kentucky. She is currently
working on her doctoral dissertation in the College of Communications and
Information Science at the University of Kentucky. In her dissertation, 'Sexual
Harassment and Communication in Higher Education: An Exploratory Study', Wall
is looking at the effects of institution communications on the incidence of
sexual harassment at institutions of higher education.
Pat Gannon-Leary is currently a Senior Research Assistant in the Department of
Information and Library Management at the University of Northumbria in Newcastle,
UK. She trained as a librarian and worked for many years in academic libraries
including those of the Universities of Sunderland and Huddersfield in the UK
and Murray State University in the USA. Gannon-Leary has an MA in film and
television studies and a PhD in communication studies. Her interests include
media representations of the British Royal Family (the theme of her doctoral
thesis); the ethics of electronic mail; and gender differences in electronic
communications.