Chapter 7
Engendered Emotion: Gender, Power,
and the Rhetoric of Emotional Control
in American Discourse
Catherine A. Lutz
From Harre, R, & Parrot, W.G., (1996) (Eds.) The
emotions, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. pp. 152-170.
In
Western academic discourse, emotions have begun to move from their culturally
assigned place at the center of the dark recesses of inner life and are being
depicted as cultural, social and linguistic operators. In the process, we can ask not only about the
cultural foundations of things construed as emotional, but about the organising
category of 'emotion' itself. One
important aspect of that category is its association with the female, so that
qualities that define the emotional also define women. For this reason, any discourse on emotion is
also, at least implicitly, a discourse on gender.
As
both an analytic and an everyday concept in the West, emotion, like the female,
has typically been viewed as something natural rather than cultural, irrational
rather than rational, chaotic rather than ordered, subjective rather than
universal, physical rather than mental or intellectual, unintended and
uncontrollable, and hence often dangerous.
This network of associations sets emotion in disadvantaged contrast to
more valued personal processes, particularly to cognition or rational thought,
and the female in deficient relation to her male other. Another and competing theme in Western
cultural renditions of emotion, however, contrasts emotion with cold
alienation. Emotion, in this view, is
life to its absence's death, is interpersonal connection or relationship to an
unemotional estrangement, is a glorified and free nature against a shackling
civilization. This latter rendition of
emotion echoes some of the fundamental ways the female has also been
'redeemed', or alternatively and more positively, construed (Lutz, 1988).
In
this chapter, I will explore how emotion has been given a gender in some
sectors of American culture and, in the process, make two related
arguments. First, I will demonstrate
that local or everyday lay discourse on emotion explicitly and implicitly draws
links among women, subordination, rebellion and emotion by examining interview
conversations conducted with a small group of American women and men. In particular, I will explore a 'rhetoric of
control' that frequently accompanies women's (and, to a lesser extent, men's)
talk about emotion, and argue that talk about the control or management of
emotion is also a narrative about the double sided nature - both weak and
dangerous - of dominated groups. Talk
about emotional control in and by women, in other words, is talk about power
and its exercise. Second, I will argue
that this and further aspects of local discourse are echoed and reproduced in
many areas of social and natural scientific discourse that deal with the
'emotional female'. Finally, I will
present a further, more syntactic analysis of the interview conversations that
contradicts at least some of the stereotypical beliefs about the relationship
between gender and emotion that these informants, as well as social science,
have voiced. This analysis looks at the
degree to which women and men might differentially use syntactic patterns that
distance, disavow, or depersonalise the experience of emotion. The failure to find systematic differences
can be taken as tentative evidence that cultural models that paint women as
more emotionally expressive or more comfortable with a discussion of their own
emotions remain surface models and do not organise discourse at more
microscopic or out-of-awareness levels.
Gender, power,
and the rhetoric of emotional control
Western
discourse on emotions constitutes them as paradoxical entities that are both a
sign of weakness and a powerful force.
On the one hand, emotion weakens the person who experiences it. It does this both by serving as a sign of a
sort of character defect (e.g., 'She couldn't rise above her emotions') and by
being a sign of at least temporary intrapsychic disorganisation (e.g., 'She was
in a fragile state' or 'She fell apart').
The person who has 'fallen apart', needless to say, is unable to
function effectively or forcefully. On
the other hand, emotions are literally physical forces that push us into
vigorous action. 'She was charged up',
we say; 'Waves of emotion shook his body'.
Women are constructed in a similar contradictory fashion as both strong
and weak (e.g., Jordanova, 1980), and I will present evidence from the
interviews mentioned earlier that when American women and men talk about emotion,
they draw on that similarity to comment on the nature of gender and power. This feature of the emotional and of the
female produces frequent discussion in the interviews of the problem of
controlling one's feelings. Such
discussion is found in both men's and women's discourse, but much more
frequently in the latter. I will show
that this talk about control of emotions is evidence of a widely shared
cultural view of the danger of both women and their emotionality. It is also talk that may mean different
things to both the speaker and the audience when it is uttered by women and by
men, and this factor will be used to help account for differences in the rate
of use of this rhetoric of control.
Although both women and men draw on a culturally available model of
emotion as something in need of control, they can be seen as often making some
different kinds of sense and claims from it.
The
material I turn to first was collected in four extended interviews on emotion
with fifteen American working- and middle-class women and men. All white, they ranged in age from the early
twenties to the mid-seventies and included a bank teller, factory worker,
college teacher, retiree, housing code inspector and stockbroker. Most were parents. The interviews were usually conducted in
people's homes, and the interviewers included myself and a number of graduate
students, most of them women. Each
person was interviewed by the same individual for all four sessions, and
although a small number of questions organised each session, every attempt was
made to have the interviews approximate 'natural conversation'. Nonetheless, it is clearly important to keep
in mind the context of the discourse to be analysed, as it was produced by a
group of people who agreed on letter and phone solicitation 'to talk about
emotion' for an audience of relative strangers who were also academics and
mostly females.
Many
people mentioned at one or several points in the interviews that they believe
women to be more emotional than men. One
example of the variety of ways this was phrased is the account one woman gave
to explain her observation that some people seem inherently to be 'nervous
types'. She remembered about her
childhood that
the female
teachers had a tendency to really holler at the kids a lot, and when I was in
class with the male teacher, it seemed like he just let things pass by and it
didn't seem to get his goat as fast, and he didn't shout at the same time the
female may have in the same instance. . . . I think emotional people get upset
faster. I do. And like with men and women, things that are
sort of important or bothering me don't bother my husband. I think that's a difference of male and
female.[1]
One theme that frequently arises in the interviews is
what can be called the 'rhetoric of control' (Rosaldo, 1977). When people are asked to talk about emotions,
one of the most common sets of metaphors used is that in which someone or
something controls, handles, copes, deals, disciplines, or manages either or
both their emotions or the situation seen as creating the emotion. For example:
I believe an
individual can exercise a great deal of control over their emotions by
maintaining a more positive outlook, by not dwelling on the negative, by trying
to push aside an unpleasant feeling. I'm
getting angry and like I said, he's over being angry, more or less dropped it
and he expects me to also. Well we don't
have the same temper, I just can't handle it that way.
And in a more poetic turn, one person mused:
sadness ...
dipping, dipping into that ... just the out-of-controlness of things.
People typically talk about controlling emotions,
handling emotional situations as well as emotional feelings, and dealing with people, situations and
emotions.
The
notion of control operates very similarly here to the way it does in Western
discourses on sexuality (Foucault, 1980).
Both emotionality and sexuality are domains whose understanding is
dominated by a biomedical model; both are seen as universal, natural impulses;
both are talked about as existing in 'healthy' and 'unhealthy' forms; and both
have come under the control of a medical or quasi-medical profession
(principally psychiatry and psychology).
Foucault has argued that popular views of sexuality - as a drive that
was repressed during the Victorian era and gradually liberated during the
twentieth century - are misleading because they posit a single essence that is
manipulated by social convention.
Rather, Foucault postulated, multiple sexualities are constantly
produced and changed. A popular
discourse on the control of emotion runs functionally parallel to a discourse
on the control of sexuality; a rhetoric of control requires a psychophysical
essence that is manipulated or wrestled with and directs attention away from
the socially constructed nature of the idea of emotion (see Abu-Lughod and
Lutz, 1990). In addition, the metaphor
of control implies something that would otherwise be out of control, something
wild and unruly, a threat to order. To
speak about controlling emotions is to replicate the view of emotions as
natural, dangerous, irrational, and physical.[2]
What
is striking is that women talked about the control of emotion more than twice
as often as did men as a proportion of the total speech each produced in the interviews.[3] To help account for this difference, we can ask what
the rhetoric of control might accomplish for the speaker and what it might say
to several audiences (see Brenneis, 1990).
At least three things can be seen to be done via the rhetoric of emotional
control: It (1) reproduces an important part of the cultural view of emotion
(and then implicitly of women as the more emotional gender) as irrational, weak
and dangerous; (2) minimally elevates the social status of the person who
claims the need or ability to self-control emotions; and (3) opposes the view
of the feminine self as dangerous when it is reversed, that is, when the
speaker denies the need for or possibility of control of emotion. Each of these suggestions can only briefly be
examined.
First,
this rhetoric can be seen as a reproduction, primarily on the part of women, of
the view of themselves as more emotional, of emotion as dangerous, and hence of
themselves as in need of control. It
does this first by setting up a boundary - that edge over which emotion that is
uncontrolled can spill. A number of
people have noted that threats to a dominant social order are sometimes
articulated in a concern with diverse kinds of boundaries (whether physical or
social) and their integrity (e.g., Martin, 1987; Scheper-Hughes and Lock,
1987). One of the most critical
boundaries that is constituted in Western psychological discourse is that
between the inside and the outside of persons; individualism as ideology is
fundamentally based on the magnification of that particular boundary. When emotion is defined, as it also is in the
West, as something inside the individual, it provides an important symbolic
vehicle by which the problem of the maintenance of social order can be
voiced. A discourse that is concerned
with the expression, control, or repression of emotions can be seen as a
discourse on the crossing back and forth of that boundary between inside and
outside, a discourse we can expect to see in more elaborate forms in periods
and places where social relations appear to be imminently overturned.
This
rhetoric of emotional control goes further than defining and then defending
boundaries, however; it also suggests a set of roles - one strong and defensive
and the other weak but invasive - that are hierarchized and linked with gender
roles. Rosaldo (1984) notes of
hierarchical societies that they seem to evince greater concern than do more
egalitarian ones with how society controls the inner emotional self and, we can
add, with how one part of a bifurcated and hierarchically layered self controls
another, The body politic, in other words, is sometimes replicated in the
social relations of the various homunculi that populate the human mind, a kind
of 'mental politic'. When cognition out
reasons and successfully manages emotion, male-female roles are
replicated. When women speak of control,
they play the roles of both super- and subordinate, of controller and
controlee. They identify their emotions
and themselves as undisciplined and discipline both through a discourse on
control of feeling. The construction of
a feminine self, this material might suggest, includes a process by which women
come to control themselves and so obviate the necessity for more coercive
outside control.
There
is the example of one woman in her late thirties; she talked about the hate she
felt for her ex-husband, who began an affair while she was pregnant and left
her with the infant, an older child and no paid employment.
So I think you
try hard not to bring it [the feeling] out 'cause you don't want that type of
thing at home with the kids, you know.
That's very bad, very unhealthy, that's no way to grow up. So I think now, maybe I've just learned to
control it and time has changed the feeling of the hate.
The woman here defines herself as someone with a
feeling of hate and portrays it as dangerous, primarily in terms of the threat
it poses to her own children, a threat she phrases in biomedical terms (i.e.,
'unhealthy'). She replicates a view that
Shields (1987) found prevalent in a survey of twentieth-century
English-language child-rearing manuals; this is the danger that mothers' (and
not fathers') emotions are thought to present to children. In addition, this woman's description of her
feelings essentializes them as states; as such, they remain passive (see
Cancian, 1987 on the feminization of love) rather than active motivators, a
point to which we will return.
In
other cases, people do not talk about themselves, but rather remind others
(usually women) of the need to control themselves. These instances also serve to replicate the
view of women as dangerously emotional.
Another woman spoke about a female friend who still grieved for a son
who had died two years previously: 'You've got to pick up and go on. You've got to try and get those feelings
under control.' (The 'you' in this statement is a complex and multivocal sign
(Kirkpatrick, 1987), and directs the admonition to control simultaneously to
the grieving woman, the female interviewer, the speaker herself, no one in
particular, and everyone in a potential audience.
A
second pragmatic effect of the rhetoric of emotional control is a claim to have
the ability to 'rise above' one's emotions or to approve of those who do. Women, more than men, may speak of control
because they are concerned about counteracting the cultural denigration of
themselves through an association with emotion.
'I think it's important to control emotions', they say, and implicitly
remind a critical audience that they have the cooler stuff it takes to be
considered mature and rational. It is
important to note that, as academics, I and the graduate students who conducted
the interviews may have been perceived as an audience in special need of such
reminders. The speakers would have been
doing this, however, by dissociating themselves from emotion rather than by
questioning the dominant view both of themselves and of emotion.
Although
women may have less access to a view of themselves as masterful individuals, a
common aspect of the cultural scheme that is
available paints them as masterfully effective with others on joint tasks,
particularly interpersonal or emotional tasks (social science versions of this
include Chodorow, 1978; Parsons and Bales, 1955). This subtly alters the meaning of the
rhetoric of control; knowledge of what the feelings are that ,need' control and
of what control should be like is perceived and described as a social rather
than an individual process. For example,
one woman says: 'If you're tied in with a family, . . . you have to use it for
guidance how you control your emotions.' This is the same woman whose central
life problem during the interview period was coping with her husband's ex-wife
and family, who lived across the street from her. The regular, friendly contact between husband
and ex-wife has left her very unhappy but also unsure about what to do. The ambiguity over who ought to control or
regulate what is evident in her description of an argument she had with her
husband over the issue.
I was mad. I was mad.
And I said, 'I don't care whether you think I should [inaudible word] or
stay in this at all, it's too, and cause I'm going to say it.' And I said, 'How
dare you tell me how I'm supposed to feel', you know. Bob [her husband] would say, you know, 'You
got to live with it' or 'You got to do this' or 'How dare you tell me this, I
don't have to put up with anything' or 'I don't have to feel this way because
you tell me I have to feel this way'.
You know, it was, in that case Robin is his ex-wife, 'and you have to
just kind of deal with it', you know, 'all the problems that she presents in
your own way'. And it was almost sort of
like saying 'You're going to have to like it'.
Well I don't. I don't, you
know. And for a year and a half he kept
saying, you know, 'You're going to have to like it, this is the way it's going
to be, you're going to have to do this, you're going to have to have, be, act,
this certain way', you know, act everything hunky-dory, and it wasn't, you
know, and I was beginning to resent a whole lot of things. 1, 1, I resented him
for telling me I had to feel that way when 1, I wasn't real fond of the
situation. I didn't like it. When I would tell him that I didn't like it,
it was 'It's your problem, you deal with it'.
I didn't like that, that made me really angry because I was saying,
'Help me out here, I don't know how to deal with this'.
This
woman is frustrated with her husband for failing to join her in a collaborative
project of 'dealing with' her feelings of resentment. Here control is given away to or shared with
others. This strategy of control is more
complex and subtle than the simple self-imposition described in other parts of
the transcripts so far; it aims to control both the emotions of the self and the attention and assistance of the
other. Note also that she speaks of
'resenting' or 'not liking' (relatively mild terms of displeasure) the overall
situation but is most incensed ('mad, mad, mad') about her husband's assumption
that she ought not to feel a certain way.
She asserts the right to 'feel' unhappy about her predicament but is
clearly defining that feeling in the standard contemporary sense of a strictly
internal and passive event. Nowhere in
the interview does she explicitly state or appear to imply that she wants,
intends, or ought to act in concert with those feelings. What is being controlled or dealt with,
therefore, has already been defined as a relatively innocuous feeling rather
than an action tendency.
Finally,
the rhetoric of emotional control can also be employed in both idiosyncratic
and 'reversed' ways that may intend or have the effect of at least minimally
resisting the dominant view of emotionality, and thus of women. A few people, for example, spontaneously
spoke about the problem of emotional control, thereby evoking the whole schema
we have just been looking at. They went
on, however, to define 'control' in a way that entailed relatively minimal
constraints on emotional communication.
One woman, a twenty-eight-year-old bank teller, said: 'Let me explain
control. It's not that you sit there and
you take it [some kind of abuse] and, you know, I think controlling them
[emotions] is letting them out in the proper time, in the proper place.' Perhaps
more radically, some women (as well as one of the gay men with whom I spoke)
denied that they had the ability to control some or many of their emotions.[4] One man in his
twenties critically described a previous tendency he had to
over-intellectualise problems and explained that he worked against that
tendency because
It wasn't that I
wanted to cut off my emotions, I just didn't, they would get out of control,
and I found that the more I tried to suppress them, the more powerful they
would become. It was like this big dam
that didn't let a little out at a time, it would just explode all of a sudden,
and I'd be totally out of control.
The question remains, however, of the validity of
seeing these latter seemingly resistant uses of the rhetoric of emotional
control as 'oppositional' forms (Williams, 1977) within that system.[5] This is
certainly a dangerous rhetorical strategy, caught as they (we) are within a
hegemonic discourse not of our own making.
The opposition to self-control will most likely be absorbed into the
logic of the existing system and so come to equal not resistance but simple
deficiency or lack (of control).[6] A possibly
oppositional intent may have collaborative outcomes to the extent that the
denial of self-control is taken by most audiences as a deficit and a
confirmation of ideas about women's irrationality.
The
culturally constructed emotionality of women is rife with contradiction. The emotional female, like the natural world
that is the cultural source of both affect and women, is constructed as both
pliant (because weak and a resource for use by civilized man) and ultimately
tremendously powerful and uncontrollable (Strathern, 1980).[7] Emotionality
is the source of women's value, their expertise in lieu of rationality, and yet
it is the origin of their unsuitability for broader social tasks and even a
potential threat to their children.
There
are vivid parallels between this and the cultural meanings surrounding
colonialism that Taussig (1984) and Stoler (1985) have described. Looking at early-twentieth-century colonists'
views of the local Colombian labour force, Taussig describes their alternation
between fear and awe of Indians who were perceived as dangerous and powerful
figures, on the one hand, and disgust and denigration of their perceived
weakness and lack of civilization, on the other. Taussig describes the process as one in which
a ,colonial mirror' 'reflects back onto the colonists the barbarity of their
own social relations' (1984: 495). In a
(certainly less systematic or universally brutal) way, a 'patriarchal mirror'
can be conceptualised as helping to produce the view of women as emotional - as
dangerously 'eruptive' and as in the process of weakly 'breaking down'. A 'paradox of will' seems consistently to attend
dominating relationships - whether those of gender, race or class - as the
subordinate other is ideologically painted as weak (so as to need protection or
discipline) and yet periodically as threatening to break the ideological
boundary in riot or hysteria. Emotion
talk, as evident in these transcripts, shows the same contradictions of
control, weakness and strength. Given
its definition as nature, at least in the West, emotion discourses may be one
of the most likely and powerful devices by which domination proceeds.
The engendering
of emotion in science
Demonstrations
of the political, moral, and cultural bases of Western science have been made
convincingly in a number of natural and social fields (e.g., Asad, 1973;
Fausto-Sterling, 1985; Haan et al., 1983; Sampson, 1981). In like fashion, it can be argued that the
sciences of emotion have been, in a significant sense, a product of their
social context. In particular, the
academic literature on emotion can be considered a form of political discourse
on gender relations because of the marked associations between the two
domains. That literature thus arises out
of and re-enters a field of power struggles for the definition of true
womanhood. As Haraway (1986) has said of
American primatology, it can be seen as 'politics by other means', and in the
case of emotions, it is most centrally a politics of gender by other
means. By examining several examples of
studies of emotion, we will see that much research over the years in biology,
psychology, sociology, sociolinguistics and other fields has been implicitly
based on everyday cultural models linking women and emotionality, and that this
research moves from the assumption of these cultural premises to their
'proof'. Most striking about these
studies is the number that naturalise the purported gender differences by
attributing them to biological or necessary and universal features of the
female role in physical and social reproduction. I will briefly examine several areas of
research, including the analysis of pre-menstrual syndrome and mood, sex
differences in the recognition of facial expressions of emotion and in
aggression, and studies of the affective components and concomitants of
motherhood. Feminist critiques of a
number of these latter fields have been intensive, and I will draw on them
while extending the analysis of the domain of emotion.
Studies
of the relationship between mood and hormonal changes have focused on women's
(rather than men's) cycles and in the process have discovered the hormonal
disease of pre-menstrual syndrome. This
syndrome is characterised by both physical pain and mood disturbances and has
been attributed by the biomedical research community to hormonal imbalances in
the women who suffer from it. The
syndrome has been used to explain a host of emotions ranging from irritability
and mood swings to depression, anxiety and panic attacks. A number of feminist critiques (Archer and
Lloyd, 1985; Fausto-Sterling, 1985; Gottlieb, 1987; Whatley, 1986) have pointed
out the weakness of the evidence for this syndrome. Assessment of women's mood is usually based
on retrospective self-report via questionnaires (one popular version being
titled the 'Menstrual Distress Questionnaire'), which allow women to draw on
cultural knowledge about the relation between gender, emotion and
hormones. Conversely, studies that
disguise the purposes of the questionnaire show no significant pre-menstrual
mood changes. The putative therapeutic
effects of hormone injections are taken as primary evidence of the female
hormonal basis for mood changes, but these studies have not been
'double-blind'. As Whatley argues, this
biomedical discourse on emotions and gender may 'cause us to ignore the fact
that our pre-menstrual mood changes ... may also correlate more closely to a
monthly cycle of low bank balances than of hormonal fluctuations' (1986:
183). Moreover, the emotional symptoms
of pre-menstrual syndrome can be seen as a discourse on both the good and the
deviant woman, on the necessity of her emotional suffering and the abnormality of,
especially, her anger or irritability (Gottlieb, 1987), both common symptoms
attached to the syndrome. Normative
academic and clinical work on pre-menstrual syndrome focuses on the
emotionality of women as both common and yet as a 'symptom' in need of a
cure. This research draws on the
entrenched cultural view of emotions as sited in females, as natural in essence
(like but independent of the 'naturalness' of females), and as irrational or
pathological when they occur.
This
line of research follows from and reinvigorates the cultural model in which
women are more emotional than men because they are more tied to the biological
processes that produce emotion. Wombs, menstruation, and hormones 'predict'
emotion. A more tacit part of the
cultural logic connecting women and emotion may arise from the view of women as
biologically inferior both because they menstruate and because they are
smaller, weaker and lack a penis. When
viewed as a form of physical chaos or 'breakdown', emotion is one other form of
biological weakness suffered by women.[8]
A number of people in the interview study just
described spontaneously articulated related ideas about the relationship
between women, hormones, emotion and pathology.
In several cases, they referred to research as the authoritative source
of their assertions, although my argument is that the relationship between
everyday and scientific ideas about women and emotion is dialectical rather
than an idea system imposed hegemonically on a previously blank or very different
lay model. According to one woman, a
forty-eight-year-old telephone operator, 'women have been known to have
different reactions to the same situation at different times of the month. And that's been a study. I've seen where some women can be downright
dangerous, they could be potential killers.'
Another
field in which some attention has been paid to sex differences is the study of
facial expression of emotion. In one
sociobiological account, female emotionality is a product of evolution. Babchuk et al. (1985) interpret studies
showing that women are better able than men to read facial expressions of
emotions in infants. In their view, this
is the result of women's long history of being the primary caretakers of
infants and the reproductive value of using these facial cues to detect infant
distress. This argument is implausible
on many grounds, not the least of which are the redundancy in infants of facial
expression and other cues to discomfort, and the theoretically at least equal
value of facial expression recognition skills for the prehistoric males, who,
in many evolutionary accounts, were engaged primarily in defending the female
and infant against threatening and dissembling outsiders. In addition, one of the central studies that
demonstrates female superiority in decoding facial expressions of emotion
(Hall, 1978) has been re-analyed and shown to account for less than 4 percent
of the variance between individuals in facial expression recognition skills
(Deaux, 1984, cited in Shields, 1987).
Despite
its obvious problems, this account of the evolution of facial expression
identification is a story with some power, as it draws on entrenched cultural
narratives about women, motherhood, children and love. Here, the first premise is that women are
more attuned to emotion in themselves and others. Unlike the pre-menstrual syndrome studies,
however, female emotionality is celebrated here, with emotions taking on their
positive sense of the interpersonally engaged, the unalienated. Women's emotionality becomes a skill and an
asset. It is significant that the
sociobiological account focuses on the use of that asset to detect distress
(rather than, for example, threat). Distress,
of course, calls for nurturance, whereas other facial expressions (in either
infants or adults) might call for flight or defense, but only the former
behavior is normative for women and mothers.
Another
line of research, on sex differences in aggression, also draws on cultural
views of emotion and women. This
happens, first, because aggression, at least in the Western cultural view, is
seen as retrospectively predictive of anger (Montagu, 1978). Anger is the one emotion that is exempted in
everyday discourse from the expectation that women feel and express more
emotion than men. It is in fact every
emotion but anger that is disapproved in men and, conversely, expected in women
(Hochschild, 1983). This gender
stereotype has been shown to have been thoroughly learned by American children
as early as the pre-school period (Bimbaum et al., 1980, cited in Shields,
1987). A recent, widely accepted, and
often cited set of studies makes the parallel claim to have demonstrated a
relationship between levels of the 'male' hormone testosterone and
aggression. Fausto-Sterling (1985)
demonstrates the weakness of the evidence for this claim and questions why it
has been taken up so enthusiastically by so many.
The
echoes of the lay view in the scientific are followed by the echoes of the
scientific view in the lay on this point as well. A professional woman in her forties in the
interview study commented on the association between aggression and gender: 'So
far the research shows that, yes, little boys are inherently more aggressive
than little girls. . . . I think it bothers me that there's a sex link with
aggression. There are a couple of
sex-linked ones that bother me but ... but I can't do anything about it.'
A
number of studies that use the cultural logic of engendered emotion focus less
on physiological differences to account for emotional ones than on universal
functions and roles. In particular, they
draw on the notion of women's reproductive role and the nurturing role and
emotions that supposedly naturally accompany it. From ethological bonding theory (Bowlby,
1969) to some schools of feminism (e.g., Ruddick, 1980), focus is placed on the
natural or inevitable emotional concomitants of motherhood (rather than
fatherhood), including particularly the positive emotions of love, caring, and
attachment. Bowlby follows the prevailing
cultural emphasis on women's emotional qualities when he focuses on the
emotions of women and their children. He
wants to explain the intensity of the bond between mother and infant, and roots
that explanation in an instinctual need for attachment in the infant and fear
of separation. Feelings of love for the
child on the part of the mother are naturalised (cf. Scheper-Hughes, 1985), and disastrous
consequences are chronicled should the infant fail to receive sufficient
quantities of mother love. These two
facets of Bowlby's approach provide the carrot and stick of natural instinct
and psychological harm to the child as reasons for continued emphasis on the
need for emotionality in women.
Ruddick
(1980), on the other hand, identifies 'resilient good humour and cheerfulness',
'attentive love' and 'humility' as among the central features of maternal
virtue that follow from (rather than precede) the task of parenting and, by
frequent correlation, the task of being female.
From these perspectives, women are more deeply embedded in relationships
with others (with the mother-infant bond as the primary example and the primary
cause). This interpersonal engagement
with others is what produces emotion, which is here defined as responses to
others with whom one is involved. From
the perspective of feminism, male individualism is antithetical to the
experience of emotion (see also Chodorow, 1978).
The
differences between these two perspectives on mothering and emotion are, of
course, crucial. Bowlby-style bonding
theory naturalises the connection between women and affect through evolutionary
theory and is continuous with earlier theorising about the elevated moral
status of women achieved through their divinely assigned and naturally embedded
mothering skills. Feminist theory most
often identifies the social division of labour rather than nature as the
ultimate source of such emotional differences.
Interestingly, however, both kinds of discourse on emotion elevate women
(the first to a domestic pedestal, the second to self-esteem and/or the ability
to resist patriarchy) by focusing on positive emotions such as love and by
using 'emotion' in its positive Romantic sense of connection and disalienation.
Yet
another view of the cultural view of women as emotional is found in the
Parsonian normative construction of family roles, in which women are the
'expressive expert' and men the 'instrumental expert' (Parsons and Bales,
1955). These competencies are seen as an
outcome of the domestic market spheres in which the genders differentially
participate. Compare this notion,
however, with the contradictory view of women's emotional impact on the family
noted in the interview example and the child-rearing manual themes described
earlier. The point may be that women are
expected to be experts in noticing and attending to the emotional needs of
others (also per Bowlby), not their own, which are rather objects of control or
suppression because they, unlike the emotions of other family members, are
defined as dangerous.
Hochschild's
(1983) important feminist revision of Parsons and Bales's scheme paints emotion
less as a skill than as a form of labour.
Women are socially assigned a much heavier burden of emotional labour
than are men. Hochschild's ideas contribute
to a breaking down of the dichotomy of emotion and thought; they can also
extend the notion of women's double day of domestic and wage labour as women
are required to contribute both emotional and cognitive labour in both paid and
unpaid spheres. In this and other
feminist analyses, gender and emotion are related through the relations of
production. For Hochschild, emotion is a
personal resource that women must self-exploit more than men. It nonetheless remains a psychophysical fact,
socially manipulated, rather than a discursive practice that constructs women
as more emotional than men.
In
sum, social science disciplines women and their psyches. It constructs emotion as an individual and
intrapsychic phenomenon and evidences the same concern as lay discourse with
the emotionality of women - its frequency, its intensity, its virtues as an
emblem of female gender identity, but most of all, its danger and implicitly
the need for its control.
Personalisation
I now return to the question of how these cultural notions about the
emotionality of women, articulated in scientific discourse, are related to
everyday discourse. The rhetoric of
control that we first looked at was shown to reflect, in multiple and complex
ways, relations of power between men and women, and to reflect them in ways
that can be said, in large measure, to reproduce the 'emotional female'. By looking closely at some more microscopic
aspects of the interview talk, however, we can see that gender differences are
minimal, a fact that may speak to the gaps and fissures in the construction of
a hegemonic discourse.
In
two of the series of interviews, people were asked, first, to describe recent
experiences with each of several common emotions and, second, to talk about how
they feel about their work and family lives.
In an analysis of a sample of 286 randomly selected interview statements
that include direct reference to emotions, I have focused on the degree to
which the statement 'personalises' the emotion experience - that is, on a
variety of ways emotions, even as they are discussed, can be distanced from the
self. It might be expected that women
would use more personalising and immediate syntactic forms if they operate
following the cultural model in which women are more emotionally expressive and
have a more emotional self-identity.
Personalisation,
or a non distancing discursive strategy, was indexed by four speech patterns
(see Table 7.1), which will now be discussed.
First,
the present tense rather than the past or conditional tense (e.g., 'I get [or am] angry whenever someone talks to me that way' compared with 'I was very angry'), is used. Tense obviously does several things to the
meaning that audiences can make of a statement about emotion. First, it can move the emotion experience
farther away from or closer to the self or another in time. Second, it can either generalise or
particularise the experience; the use of the present tense, for example, can
often include the implication that the emotion is habitually experienced by the
subject. On both of these counts, the
stereotype would lead us to expect more use of the present tense by women
speakers. In fact, there is no
difference between male and female speakers in the interview sample in the use
of the present tense. If anything, men
as a group make slightly (insignificantly) more use of it.
Second,
another element of a personalising strategy might include the use of syntactic
patterns that more directly portray the speaker as the experiencer of the
emotion. Statements were coded as
portraying the experiencer as the self, as another person (male, female, or
gender unspecified), or as leaving the experiencer unspecified (e.g., 'It was a very strong feeling of hate' or
'And that developed a certain amount
of hate toward that individual because of the fact that he . . .') or the
emotion as an abstract entity with no particular experiencer (e.g., 'Well, hate
and frustration usually go hand in hand, I would say' or "'Love"
would be, I think, a good catchall phrase because . . .'). The category of self
was further broken down by whether the self was portrayed as subject or object
of the emotion experience (e.g., 'I'm very
anxious about it' compared with 'It's making me angry just talking about
this'). The belief in women's
emotionality might lead to the expectation that women would more often portray
the self (particularly the self as subject rather than object) as the
experiencer of emotion, whereas men would portray the other as the experiencer
or leave the latter ambiguous.
Table 7.1 Personalisation in syntax
1 Present tense
'I get [or am] angry whenever someone talks to me that way.'
Others
'I was very angry.'
2 Experiencer of the emotion discussed
Self, as subject of emotion experience
'I'm very anxious about it.'
Self, as object of emotion experience
'It's making me
angry just talking about this.'
Other person (male, female, or gender unspecified)
'My father was very annoyed with me for going into that field.'
Unspecified
'It was a very strong feeling of hate.'
'And that developed
a certain amount of hate toward that individual because of the fact that he . .
None - emotion as an abstract entity
'Well, hate and frustration usually go hand in hand, I
would say.' "'Love" would be, I think a good catchall phrase because
. .
3 Cause or elicitor of emotion
Self
'They were angry at me.''
'I just kind of giggled and made her even angrier.'
Other person
'I hate her because
she was mean enough to tell me that.' 'I'm deathly afraid of dentists.'
Event
'The most anxious moment I had ... was my first performance with the ... Choral Society.'
'I hate going
out unless I really have to.'
Object
'He loves books'.'
Unspecified
'Lots of little
things are frustrating.'
'I can't talk anymore, I start screaming to begin
with, when I'm really angry.'
4 Negation
'I [or she] wasn't
angry.'
In
the interview sample, it is not significantly more common for women, in their
discussions of emotion, to focus on the experiencing self as the subject versus
the object of the emotion, nor is it more common for men to leave the
experiencer unspecified or abstract. In
addition, neither women nor men are more likely to portray others as opposed to
the self as the experiencer of the discussed emotion. Women and men speak more alike than differently
in this sample when discussing the experiencer of emotions.
Third,
statements about emotion usually contain an implicit or explicit etiology, that
is they specify the cause (usually by specifying the object) of the
feeling. Personalising strategies might
include identification of either the self or, secondarily, another person as
the ultimate cause of the emotion (rather than the use of syntactic patterns
that obscure or fail to identify the cause).
Statements were coded as portraying the cause as either the self (e.g.,
'They were angry at me' or 'I just kind of giggled and made her even
angrier'), another person (e.g., 'I hate her
because she was mean enough to tell me that' or 'I'm deathly afraid of dentists'), an event (e.g., 'The most
anxious moment I had ... was my first
performance with the ... Choral
Society' or 'I hate going out unless
I really have to'), an object (e.g., 'He loves
books'), or as leaving the cause unspecified (e.g., 'Lots of little things are frustrating' or 'I can't talk anymore, I
start screaming to begin with, when I'm really angry') (cf. Shimanoff, 1983). Given the associations between gender and
affect I noted earlier, we might expect that women more than men would see
other people as intimately involved in their own emotion experience and
themselves as evoking emotion in others, rather than seeing events as
triggering emotion in themselves or failing to specify a cause. The latter strategy can be associated with
the view of emotion as nonsensical, irrational, or without ascertainable
cause. In fact, there are no significant
gender differences in the use of personal versus impersonal causal attribution,
nor do women use self versus other attributions more than men.
Finally,
a number of statements about emotion in the interviews are essentially denials
of emotion in the self or the other (e.g., 'I [or she] wasn't angry'). The stereotype might lead us to expect more
negation in general from men and more negation of particular kinds of
female-linked emotions (which include most emotions except anger) by men and of
male-linked emotions (notably anger) by women.
Here again, women's and men's speech are indistinguishable in terms of
the proportion of emotion states that are negated as they are discussed.
The
absence of extensive differences might be attributed to the special nature of
the people interviewed, all of whom agreed beforehand to talk with a stranger
about emotion. The results are
consistent, however, with a study of gender differences in emotion language used
by Shimanoff (1983), who did a similar analysis of the tape-recorded natural
conversations of a number of American college students and married couples, and
found few differences in male and female conversations that included reference
to emotions.[9] The results are also consistent with the trend in
studies of psychological and linguistic sex differences in general, which have
tended to show far fewer differences than researchers both expected - on the
basis of cultural stereotypes about distinctive male and female styles of
thinking, behavior, and speech - and then often found in self-fulfilling
fashion. The absence of differences is
more significant given the syntactic nature of the evidence examined; Shibamoto
(1987) has concluded that gender differences that are not a response to
audience expectations about particular gender identities are more likely to be
found in syntactic patterns of use because they are typically outside of our
awareness and hence of our easy manipulation, unlike semantic patterns such as
those having to do with the notion of 'control' we examined earlier.
Conclusion
In
all societies, body disorders - which emotion is considered to be in this
society - become crucial indicators of problems with social control and, as
such, are more likely to occur or emerge in a discourse concerning social
subordinates. Foucault has made the
claim that power creates sexuality and its disciplining; similarly, it can be
said to create emotionality. The
cultural construction of women's emotion can thus be viewed not as the
repression or suppression of emotion in men (as many lay people, therapists,
and other commentators argue) but as the creation of emotion in women. Because emotion is constructed as relatively
chaotic, irrational, and antisocial, its existence vindicates authority and
legitimates the need for control. By
association with the female, it vindicates the distinction between and
hierarchy of men and women. And the
cultural logic connecting women and emotion corresponds to and shores up the
walls between the spheres of private, intimate (and emotional) relations in the
(ideologically) female domain of the family and public, formal (and rational)
relations in the primarily male domain of the marketplace.
Rubin
has remarked of sexuality that 'There are historical periods in which [it] is
more sharply contested and more overtly politicised' (1984: 267). Emotionality has the same historical
dynamism, with shifting gender relations often appearing to be at the root of
both academic and lay struggles over how emotion is to be defined and
evaluated.[10] In other words, the contemporary dominant discourse
on emotions - and particularly the view that they are irrational and to be
controlled - helps construct but does not wholly determine women's discourse;
there is an attempt to recast the association of women with emotion in an
alternative feminist voice.
Feminist
treatments of the question of emotion (e.g., Hochschild, 1983; Jagger, 1987)
have tended to portray emotions not as chaos but as a discourse on
problems. Some have contested both the
irrationality and the passivity of feelings by arguing that emotions may
involve the identification of problems in women's lives and are therefore
political. Talk about anger, for
example, can be interpreted as an attempt to identify the existence of
inappropriate restraint or injustice.
Sadness is a discourse on the problem of loss, fear on that of
danger. By extension, talk about the
control of emotions would be, in this feminist discourse, talk about the
suppression of public acknowledgement of problems. The emotional female might then be seen not
simply as a mythic construction on the axis of some arbitrary cultural dualism
but as an outcome of the fact that women occupy an objectively more problematic
position than does the white, upper-class, Northern European, older man who is
the cultural exemplar par excellence of cool, emotionless rationality. According to a feminist analysis, whether or
not women express their problems (i.e., are emotional) more than men, those
women's audiences may hear a message that is an amalgam of the orthodox view
and its feminist contestation: 'We (those) women are dangerously close to
erupting into emotionality/pointing to a problem/ moving toward a social
critique.'
References
Abu-Lughod, Lila (1986) Veiled Sentiments.. Honour and Poetry in a Bedouin Society. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Abu-Lughod, Lila and Lutz, Catherine A. (1990)
'Introduction: emotion, discourse, and the politics of everyday life', in C.A.
Lutz and L. Abu-Lughod (eds), Language
and the Politics of Emotion. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Archer, John and Lloyd, Barbara (1985) Sex and Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Asad, Talal (ed.) (1973) Anthropology and the Colonial Encounter. New York: Humanities Press.
Babchuk, Wayne, Hames, Raymond and Thompson, Ross
(1985) 'Sex differences in the recognition of infant facial expressions of
emotion: The primary caretaker hypothesis', Ethology
and Sociobiology, 6: 89 101.
Bimbaum, D.A., Nosanchuck, T.A. and Croll, W.L. (1980)
'Children's stereotypes about sex differences in emotionality', Sex Roles, 6: 435-43.
Bowlby, John (1969) Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1. London: Hogarth Press.
Brenneis, D. (1990) 'Shared and solitary sentiments',
in C.A. Lutz and L. Abu-Lughod (eds), Language
and the Politics of Emotion. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. Chapter Six.
Cancian, Francesca (1987) Love in America: Gender and Self-Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chodorow, Nancy (1978) The Reproduction of Mothering. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Deaux, K. (1984) 'From individual differences to
social categories: Analysis of a decade's research on gender', American Psychologist, 39: 105-16.
Fausto-Sterling, Anne (1985) Myths of Gender.. Biological Theories of Women and Men. New York: Basic Books.
Foucault, Michel (1980) The History of Sexuality, Vol. 1. New York: Vintage.
Gottlieb, Alma (1987) 'American PMS: A mute
voice'. Paper presented at the annual
meetings of the American Anthropological Association, Chicago.
Haan, Norma, Bellah, Robert, Rabinow, Paul and
Sullivan, William (1983) Social Science
as Moral Inquiry. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Hall, J. (1978)
'Gender effects in decoding non-verbal cues', Psychological Bulletin, 85: 845-75.
Haraway, Donna (1986) 'Primatology is politics by other means', in Ruth
Bleier (ed.), Feminist Approaches to
Science. New York: Pergamon Press.
pp. 77-118.
Hochschild, Arlie (1983) The Managed Heart: Commercialisation of Human Feeling. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Holland, Dorothy and Quinn, Naomi (eds) (1987) Cultural Modes in Language and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jagger, Alison (1987) 'Love and knowledge: Emotion as
an epistemic resource for feminists',
Ms. in possession of author. Department of Philosophy, University of
Cincinnati.
Jordanova, L.J. (1980) 'Natural facts: A historical
perspective on science and sexuality', in Carol MacCormack and Marilyn
Strathern (eds), Nature, Culture, and
Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press. pp. 42 69.
Kirkpatrick, John (1987) 'Representing the self as
'You' in American discourse'. Paper
presented at the annual meetings of the American Anthropological Association,
Chicago.
Lutz, Catherine (1988) Unnatural Emotions: Everyday Sentiments on a Micronesian Atoll and
Their Challenge to Western Theory. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Maher, Vanessa (1984) 'Possession and dispossession:
Maternity and mortality in Morocco', in H. Medick and D. Sabean (eds), Interest and Emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
pp. 103-28.
Martin, Emily (1987) 'The ideology of reproduction:
The reproduction of ideology'. Paper
presented to the Upstate New York Feminist Scholars' Network, September.
Montagu, Ashley (ed.) (1987) The Learning of Non-Aggression. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Parsons, Talcott and Bales, Robert (1955) Family, Socialization, and Interaction
Process. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Rosaldo, Michelle Z. (1 984) 'Toward an anthropology
of self and feeling', in R. Shweder and R. Le Vine (eds), Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self and Emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
pp. 137-57.
Rosaldo, Renato (1977) 'The rhetoric of control:
Ilongots viewed as natural bandits and wild Indians', in B. Babcock (ed.), The Reversible World.. Symbolic Inversion in
Art and Society. Ithaca, NY: Cornell
University Press. pp. 240-57.
Rubin, Gayle (1984) 'Thinking sex; Notes for a radical
theory of the politics of sexuality', in Carol S. Vance (ed.), Pleasure and Danger: Exploring Female
Sexuality. Boston: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Ruddick, Sara (1980) 'Maternal thinking', Feminist Studies, 6: 70 96.
Sampson, E.E. (1981) 'Cognitive psychology as
ideology', American Psychologist, 36:
730-43. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy (1985)
'Culture, scarcity, and maternal thinking', Ethos,
13: 291-317. Scheper-Hughes, Nancy
and Lock, Margaret (1987) 'The mindful body: A prolegomenon to future work in
medical anthropology', Medical
Anthropology Quarterly, 1: 6-41.
Shibamoto, Janet (1987) 'The womanly woman:
Manipulation of stereotypical and non stereotypical features of Japanese female
speech', in S. Philips, S. Steele and C. Tanz (eds), Language, Gender, and Sex in Comparative Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
pp. 26-49.
Shields, Stephanie A. (1987) 'Women, men and the
dilemma of emotion', in P. Shaver and C. Hendrick (eds), Sex and Gender. Newbury
Park, CA: Sage Publications. pp. 229 50.
Shimanoff, Susan (1983) 'The role of gender in
linguistic references to emotive states', Communication
Quarterly, 30: 174-9.
Smith-Rosenberg, Carroll (1972) 'The hysterical woman:
Roles and role conflict in 19th-century America', Social Research, 39: 652 78.
Stoler, Anne (1985) 'Perceptions of protest: Defining
the dangerous in colonial Sumatra', American
Ethnologist, 12: 642-58.
Strathern, Marilyn (1980) 'No nature, no culture: The
Hagen case', in Carol MacCormack and Marilyn Strathern (eds), Nature, Culture, and Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 174-222.
Taussig, Michael (1984) 'Culture of terror - Space of
Death: Roger Casement's Putumayo report and the explanation of torture', Comparative Studies in Society and History, 26:
467-97.
Whatley, Marianne (1986) 'Taking feminist science to
the classroom: Where do we go from here?', in Ruth Bleier (ed.), Feminist Approaches to Science. New York: Pergamon Press. pp. 181-90.
Williams, Raymond (1977) Marxism and Literature. Oxford:
Oxford University Press. Woolard,
Kathryn (1985) 'Language variation and cultural hegemony: toward an integration
of sociolinguistic and social theory', American
Ethnologist, 12: 738-48.
Notes
An earlier version of this chapter was presented on
the panel 'Emotion and Discourse' at the annual meeting of the American
Anthropological Association, Chicago, 18-22 November 1987. The draft has benefited greatly from the
comments of Lila Abu-Lughod and Steven Feld.
The research on which this chapter is based has been conducted with
grants from the State University of New York Foundation and the National
Institute of Mental Health and with the help of many people. Kathryn Beach, Robin Brown, Paula Bienenfeld
and Walter Komorowski assisted in interviewing and transcription, and the
expert analytic work of Angela Carroll and Marion Pratt helped give the chapter
its form.
[1] The actual
process by which these models of gender and emotion are acquired is a
fascinating but unexplored question. We
might expect that it includes, in part, the child's reasoning from the
culturally assigned authority and control of the male teacher to a lack of
emotion, the latter perhaps already having been learned to require 'strength'
and 'control' to master - in other words, to generalise from the dominant
position of males to a presumed lack of emotion (a process that might also have
occurred in her teachers' views of themselves).
[2] The method used
in looking at the transcripts draws on recent developments in the ,cognitive'
study of cultural meaning. These focus
on the analysis of extended and relatively natural conversations for the
cultural knowledge or cultural models (Holland and Quinn, 1987) evident, if not
always explicitly stated, in them. By
looking at such things as syntax, metaphor, or the prepositional networks
underlying the sensibility of sentence order, it is possible to draw inferences
about the kinds of models individuals are using or, perhaps more aptly, to draw
inferences about the kinds of inferences listeners can make about what the
speaker has left unsaid but likely wants understood.
[3] There are 180
instances in those parts of the women's transcripts analysed so far, and 85
instances in the men's, with each set of transcripts being of approximately equal
length.
[4] 1 have found
Woolard's (1985) analysis of the nature of hegemonic and oppositional forms of
language use very productive in formulating what I have to say here.
[5] Martin (1987)
has examined the American discourses on reproduction and women's bodies and has
rigorously uncovered the contradiction between a view of uterine contractions
during childbirth as involuntary and a view of the woman as in fact in control
of the labour process. The women she interviewed
about their birth experiences spoke very similarly to the women described in
this chapter about their sense of control over the physical process and over
their cries of pain and pleasure during labour and birth. She notes a class difference, however, with
middle-class women speaking with more approval of control than working-class
women. We might then expect men also to
express more concern with and approval of control of emotion, which is not the
case here. This is certainly a problem
worthy of more study, particularly a delineation of what kinds of control of
which domains appear to emerge from what kinds of experience within
hierarchical systems.
[6] Acknowledgement
of one's emotionality may mean very different things to female and male
audiences. Women may announce to each
other shared identity and solidarity, while asserting difference, submission,
or defiance when making similar statements to men.
[7] Abu-Lughod's
(1986) study of the Awlad 'Ali represents the most detailed and eloquent
example of how, in another cultural system, the particular kinds of emotions allocated to and voiced by women articulate with
other aspects of their ideological and social structural positions.
[8] This group of
studies obviously follows in the tradition of centuries of expert explanations
of hysteria. Although there have been
many versions of the explanation (such as one nineteenth-century account that
diagnosed its origins as an empty womb and a childhood where the restraint of
emotion was not taught [Smith-Rosenberg, 1972]), they have been organised
around the connection between female physiology and mood.
[9] Shimanoff (1983)
found that male and female speakers did not differ in the number of affect
words they used, in the tense, valence, or source (similar to the notion of
'elicitor' used here) of statements about emotion. She did find, however, that males made more
reference to their own emotions than to those of other people when compared
with females.
[10] The resurgence
of interest in emotion in the late 1970s and 1980s across the social sciences
may in part be the result of the feminist movement's revalorization of all
things traditionally associated with women (Margaret Trawick, personal
communication). Changing gender
relations may also be at the root of the reinvigoration of a long-standing
Western discourse on the value of emotional expression; the current debate pits
expressionists, for whom healthy emotions are vented ones, against those who
would dismiss the latter as 'self-indulgent' or 'immature'. This debate no doubt draws in a complex way,
in each concrete context in which it occurs, on the gender ideologies and
conflicts of the individual participants.