Interviewing
From: Banister, P., Burman, E., Parker, I., Taylor, M. & Tindall, C. (1994) Qualitative methods in psychology: A research guide, Buckingham: Open University Press
Conducting
interviews is a complex, labour-intensive and uncertain business, fraught with
tricky issues that social scientific researchers, and particularly
psychologists, are often ill-equipped to address. This is because the emphasis
on detachment and the distance structured between researcher and researched
within most psychological theory and research instruments is rudely challenged
by the face-to-face research interview. However much this is warded off by
professional and personal defences, an interview is at some level inevitably a
personal and sometimes intimate, as well as public, encounter. This chapter
briefly outlines the rationale for doing interviews and different theoretical
models of the interviewing process. The focus here is on the researcher's role,
and the analysis section will draw on examples of interviewer and interviewee
relations to elaborate this further. At the outset I should make it clear that,
although guidelines for good practice are presented here, as with some other
qualitative research methods, the diverse and specific nature of interviews
means that no blueprint of interviewing practice or analysis can be absolutely
determined in advance and in abstraction from the topic and context of a
particular inquiry. Rather, the aim here is to highlight some of the issues
that need to be addressed when designing, conducting and analysing
interview-based research. It should also be noted that this chapter, as a
reflection of most of the research literature and practice, is concerned with
one-to-one, face-to-face interviews. While questions about the conduct of the
research and the power of the researcher discussed here also apply to group
discussions (which are increasingly gaining recognition as contexts of
research, from market research to action research), these may be either
magnified or mitigated by distinctive group processes.
While the content of this chapter covers
issues sometimes addressed under the titles of 'social research' or
'ethnography', we are dealing here with what might broadly be called
'semi-structured' approaches to interviewing. We will use the term 'thematic
analysis' for the process of making sense of the interview. There is a double
contrast implied in the account of interviewing practice presented here: with
structured approaches, which are usually quantitative and closer to questionnaires
in structuring the interviewee's responses, and with so-called unstructured
approaches, which for reasons argued below we regard as at best a disingenuous
and sometimes a dangerous misnomer for refusing to acknowledge prior
expectations or agendas. The position taken here is that assumptions structure
all research, and the least we can do is to recognise this and theorise the
impact of these assumptions. Better still, we can plan and articulate our
starting assumptions so as to scrutinise and promote the research goals. Accordingly,
the questions raised in this chapter tie in closely with the discussion in
Chapter 8 on feminist research, and some of the more critical issues about
interviewing practice are taken up there.
Background
There
are four main reasons for conducting interviews. First, uniting the many models
of interviewing is a concern with subjective meanings (the meanings the
participants accord to the topic of the interview) rather than with eliciting
responses within a standard format for comparison with other individuals or
groups.
Second, interviews can permit exploration
of issues that may be too complex to investigate through quantitative means.
That is, given the latter's aim to simplify phenomena, they can misrepresent
the nature of the questions under investigation. For example, if you wanted to
explore roles, relationships and ethics with a particular professional group,
or even to compare perceptions of a service between service providers and
recipients, it is unlikely that you would gain a sufficiently sensitive and
incisive grasp of your participants' concerns by administering a questionnaire
with rating scale categories. This might be not so much because the scale does
not address the correct questions (although this might also be true), as
because the views of the participants cannot be readily representable within
that form. Hence, holding inconsistent, contradictory views is not necessarily
a function of faulty reasoning, but rather may be a reflection of the real
contradictions and complexities of the way
the service works in practice.
Your
aim in using a semi-structured interview may be to explore precisely those
areas where your interviewee perceives gaps, contradictions and difficulties.
Hence another advantage of using a less structured approach is that you can
tailor your questions to the position and comments of your interviewee, and you
are not bound by the codes of standardisation and replicability to soldier on
through your interview schedule irrespective of how appropriate it is for your
interviewee.
Within this approach, then, you should
respond to and follow up issues raised by your interviewee, including ones that
you may not have anticipated. In this sense, semi-structured interviewing, as a
more open and flexible research tool, can document perspectives not usually
represented (or even envisaged by researchers), and hence the approach can
empower disadvantaged groups by validating and publicising their views (e.g.
Mishler 1986). While in an experiment the key question is specified in advance
as the hypothesis (or so the story goes), and that question is (supposedly) the
only question that the experiment addresses, in an interview the focus of the
interview can be (although is not necessarily) a matter of negotiation.
Third, doing interviews is a salutary
lesson in research involvement and practice. Without the 'safe' distance of a
one-way mirror or the position of the detached manipulator of variables, as an
interviewer one is forced to confront one's own participation within the
research. We can take this point further to reflect on whether this lesson is
particular to interviewing, or whether questions of the social construction of
research 'data' reverberate further with implications for all empirical work.
At any rate, conducting interviews demands consideration of reflexivity in the
research process, extending from the devising of the research question, to
identifying and setting up interviews with informants, to the interview itself
(your role, how you were seen by the interviewee, your reflections on the
process), and including the work done to transform an interactive encounter
into a piece of written research.
Fourth,
necessarily associated with the process of making visible your own work in the
construction of your material, is the question of power relations in research.
An early account of interviewing (Bingham and Moore 1959) describes the
interview as a 'conversation with a purpose'. We should stop to consider whose purposes the conversation is pursuing.
Research sets up, and is conducted within, power relationships. We need to
attend to these in terms of both the morality-politics of research practice and
the academic criteria of adequate evaluation of research (though such a strict
separation is of course impossible to maintain once we consider these issues).
Some models of research relationships try to do more than acknowledge the
structural power relationships set up by research (see the chapters on feminist
research and action research), to mitigate, challenge or even reverse
traditional power dynamics. The move from designating the people who form the
focus of the research as 'subjects' to 'interviewees' or ‘participants' or
'informants' or 'co-researchers' reflects attempts to do research 'with' rather
than 'on' people. Of course, the research relationship is only one of various
structural power relations that can enter into the research. We should also,
therefore, consider the extent to which class, 'race', gender and age relations
(for example) interact with the interviewing relationship. Again we can
reflect on the extent to which these issues are specific to interviewing, or,
although perhaps particularly visible here, are just as relevant to other forms
of research practice. Nevertheless, we need to maintain an 'interpretive
vigilance' (Figueroa and Lopez, 1991) to ward off the ways researcher control
is implicitly structured and exercised within research instruments claiming to
be participative and consultative.
Models of interviewing
Broadly
speaking, four approaches inform interviewing practice: ethnographic, 'new
paradigm', feminist and postmodernist. While these approaches can overlap and
combine, each has its own language and way of conceiving the research process
and relationship. So while they have much in common with each other, it is
worth identifying a few contrasts or points of tension within interviewing
style and interpretation here. In all approaches, however, reflexivity is accorded a key role, in the sense of the researcher
reflecting on her or his own experience and role within the conduct of the
research.
While ethnographic work highlights
informants' expertise and the dependence of the researcher on the informant
for access to her or his subjective rules, meanings and cultural life, there
is a clear role demarcation between researcher and researched in determining
the research topic and outcome (although this is changing within contemporary
anthropological work: see Nencel and Pels 1991). Further, notwithstanding its
ethos of eliciting and representing descriptions, we should not lose sight of
how even ethnographic work still requires prior identification and structuring
of themes to be investigated. On this, James Spradley (1979: 55) provides a clear account of the differences
between an ethnographic interview and an 'ordinary conversation . There are
similarities here with Jean Piaget's clinical interview process, where it is
argued that 'the good practitioner lets himself [sic] be led, though always in control, and takes account of the
whole of the mental context' (Piaget 1929: 19).
In contrast, in 'new paradigm' research
(Reason and Rowan 1981), while following the ethos of valuing what people say
and treating this as meaningful and informative, research is viewed as a
collaborative enterprise which not only involves the full participation of the
interviewees but
also
incurs responsibility on the part of the researcher to be accountable to, and
in some cases to conduct research agendas according to the demands of, the
participants (see Chapter 1). Here we see the traditional model of
researcher-researched relations undergoing upheaval as the researcher strives
to carry out research in a non-exploitative, non-dehumanising way.
Discussions of feminist methodology also
take as central issues of power in the conduct of research. But rather than
focusing only on the interpersonal relationship set up within the research
encounter, feminist approaches attend in addition to wider questions of power
as they enter into the funding, popularisation and uses of research (e.g.
Spender 1981). Moreover, they often treat power not as something that can be
removed from research, but rather as an ever-present dynamic that needs to be
acknowledged as structuring the interaction in diverse ways. In this sense
feminist analyses of power in terms of the social positions occupied by
interviewees, and (re)produced within interviews, go beyond those offered in
'new paradigm' accounts - most noticeably, but not exclusively, in terms of
gender.
Finally, there are accounts of research
drawing on post-structuralist and postmodernist writings to critique
traditional models of research. This might include social constructionist and
narrative approaches to research (e.g. Mishler 1986; Steier 1991). Of
particular relevance here is the questioning of the presumption that
participants within research share the research goals. The changes to which
the research is directed may well be worthy, but may be of no immediate benefit
to the informant at whose expense careers are gained and whose experience is
subordinated to a preconceived or more or less imposed interpretive framework
(see Gubrium and Silverman 1989; Opie 1992). Critiques along these lines invite
attention to the variety of interpretations that can and will be made by
different parties to the research encounter, and therefore also call for a
principled scrutiny of our work of interpretation as researchers. In addition,
more transformative research practice would seek to identify and address this
multiplicity of interpretations in terms of research goals.
Constructing and selecting interview material
It
is worth remembering that work done before the actual conduct of the interview
is usually amply repaid in terms of its success and ease of analysis. First,
you will have arrived at a topic to research, but you should clarify the
rationale for doing this. Second, you should specify who would best exemplify
the perspectives or range of perspectives relevant to your research question.
Third, you should generate an interview schedule. At early stages in the
planning of the research this may simply be a list of headings which you can
elaborate in more detail once you have sorted out who your participants are,
but it is worth doing this work now so that you have a clearer focus for when
you approach your participants. Fourth, now that you have decided what kinds of
people you want to interview, you need to contact them. It is very important to
consider the impact of the route by which you contacted your participants in
terms of how this structures the ways they see you, so that, for example, if
you are interested in experiences of social or health services delivery it may
be difficult to dispel the image of being associated with evaluation or
treatment if you initially approach them via a medical or legal agency. It may,
however, be impossible to avoid such constraints, but you should at least
theorise how this may limit the form and content of the accounts you elicit.
What your prospective interviewees see the
study as being about will also be central to their decision about whether to
participate, and, in line with codes of practice about 'informed consent', you
should be as open as possible about your aims. This may include outlining the
kinds of areas or questions you want to discuss with them, and this information
can do much to allay participants' anxieties or reservations. You should also
at this point discuss what records you want to make of the interview, as in
seeking permission to audiotape, for example; it might be helpful to explain
why this is useful and how you will use it. Fifth, at this point you should
negotiate a research contract with your participant, which includes guarantees
of anonymity, a promise to terminate the interview at any point if the
interviewee feels uncomfortable, the exclusion from the transcript or other
records of anything the interviewee does not wish to be seen by others and a
copy of the final report if desired.
While all this may gain you your
participants (and if people refuse, you should consider why this may be), you
now need to plan the interview itself. First, you will need to elaborate your
interview schedule. In qualitative interviews it may not be appropriate to ask
your interviewees similar questions; indeed, the 'same' question may have a far
from equivalent meaning depending on the interview context, the interviewee's
position and the research relationship. Since what you are interested in here
is divergence and variety, rather than convergence and replicability, you may
be better able to address your general aims by orienting the question to the
particular positions of your participants.
Some
people like to prepare a detailed interview schedule, with questions
addressing all the key issues they want to cover. While this can be reassuring
for the researcher, it needs to be treated flexibly in the interview itself
since too rigid adherence can intimidate the participant or can fail to follow
the participant's train of associations and perspectives. It can therefore be
more helpful to have a list of topic areas, with lists of issues you want to
cover, arranged so that it is easy for you to check them out in the course of
the interview. But in this case the danger is that, while responding to the
particular context and moment to ask your question, you either betray too much
of your own perspective in the formulation you use or, in the heat of the
moment, are lost for words. This is why it is useful to pose topic headings in
the form of questions so that you do not have to do so much thinking on your
feet. In general you should ask open questions, not only in the sense of
avoiding questions that can be answered with a simple yes or no (unless you
follow this up with a 'Can you say a bit more about that?'), but also avoiding
formulations that could be interpreted as prescriptions for, or prohibitions
on, what can be talked about - unless you consider that the situation or topic
merits you positioning yourself more clearly.
Second, while these recommendations may
seem daunting, all becomes much clearer and easier when you do a practice
interview, perhaps with a friend with whom you feel at ease and who can give
you frank feedback on the content and process of the interview. This helps to
identify and iron out problems with the interview schedule and with the
recording equipment (so that you remember to switch it on, you know where to
position the microphone, you know not to have the machine on autoreverse so
that it records over the first side again, etc.). Not least, you will gain a
lot of confidence from the experience, even if you are also made acutely aware
of the demands made on you as interviewer. These include the capacities to
focus in parallel on listening intently to what your interviewee says, reflecting
on how this relates to your interests, preconceptions and schedule, and working
out what to say and when to say it.
Example
The
sample analysis below reflects the focus so far on reflexive issues. It draws
on aspects of all models identified above, but is most influenced by feminist
and postmodernist approaches. In looking at this thematic analysis it is
important to be clear that, even when carefully selected, abstracted from its
original context and juxtaposed with other examples, as are the extracts below,
they do not 'speak for themselves'. Meaning inheres not only in the text but in
our construction and reading of it: despite the process of selection and
interpretation in the preparation of this illustrative material, the analysis
is inevitably selective. We are going to illustrate features of interviewing
practice through a focus on instances of metacommentary (commentary on, in this
case, the research process) occurring within some extracts from interview
transcripts. At the outset we need to identify the questions in relation to
which the analysis is structured, present the rationale for the material used
and introduce the material itself
Analytic questions
The
questions selected here concern the visibility of rules governing the structure
of the interview, which here are exhibited through moments of role-shifts and
changes between interviewer and participant. Through this analysis we aim to
illustrate how participants hold and use their positions within the research
relationship. That is, they are neither passive nor unknowing about their
positioning, but rather use this to achieve specific outcomes within the
interview. We would not claim that these examples are routinely found within
interview transcripts, but they are certainly not unique. The fact that these
examples originate from interviews in which all the interviewers knew their
informants beforehand (and indeed presumably selected them as willing to talk,
and to talk about the selected topic) perhaps enabled the expression of what is
often either non-verbally or indirectly communicated, or commented on off-tape.
In line with current reflexive work on research practice, the focus here is on
making visible the researcher within the research context. Correspondingly, for
purposes of exposition we are going to exaggerate the reflexive ethos to focus
not on the individual interview, or interviewer, but on what these extracts can
tell us about research processes (which also includes what we are doing with
them here).
Construction and selection of interview
material
The
extracts below are drawn from three second-year undergraduate practicals where
students were conducting a single semi-structured interview supervised by one
of us on a topic of their choice. As is appropriate research practice, they
negotiated a research contract with the participant before the interview
itself, which included ensuring that the transcripts would remain anonymous and
would be read only by them and the markers of the work (of which, as the
supervisor, I was one). Students were encouraged to discuss the interview
process with participants to promote reciprocity and feedback. Similarly, these
are the conditions under which I am drawing on these interviews. I am
reproducing these extracts with the students' permission and with that of their
informants. The fact that I did not conduct these interviews, and moreover am
presenting a necessarily selective and motivated analysis, is an issue that I
flag here to take up later.
Material
It
is a moot point whether the material for analysis here is the text of the
interview or the interview itself. Frequently researchers take as their record,
as their 'raw material' for analysis, the transcript of their interview. This
is belied by the fact that a transcript is: (a) an impoverished record, a key
stopping point on the road of progressive removal from encounter, to aural
representation (on tape), to written representation; and (b) therefore a
selective/constructive representation, as highlighted by the variety of
transcription notations that embody their own assumptions, whether through
spatial arrangement (Ochs 1979) or through levels of detail about what is
important (discussed briefly in Stubbs 1983, and extensively in Tedlock 1984).
Furthermore, not only is the production of
a transcript also part of the research process, but the interviewer brings to
the transcript her experience and memory of the interview. Here too it is
appropriate to articulate impressions and perceptions of emergent issues and
feelings, preferably as soon after the interview as possible, and in any case
before starting on the analysis. These 'field notes' can become a resource,
both in informing the analysis and in reminding you of what assumptions you
brought to that analysis - which may or may not be borne out. Given all this
construction and selection in the process of organisation and synthesis that
is necessary for an analysis, we should also question what is excluded, suppressed.
The material presented here for analysis is of a variety that in many accounts
would be sanitised away, deemed bad research, embarrassing intrusions of the
personal or even lapses of interviewer control. These are precisely what make
them interesting and important to analyse as indicative of the implicit rules
of research, rules which become more apparent in their infraction.
Analysis
A
'thematic analysis' is a coherent way of organising or reading some interview
material in relation to specific research questions. These readings are
organised under thematic headings in ways that attempt to do justice both to
the elements of the research question and to the preoccupations of the
interviewees. I will start by presenting each extract separately and then move
on to elaborate connections and contrasts between them in relation to the
themes of power relations in research, the power of the participant and shifts
in positions. The texts are longer here, for illustrative purposes, than would
normally be presented within the main text of a report. The transcription and
terminology (I, interviewer; R, respondent; P, participant) are as in the
originals. Clearly these terms reflect different ways of positioning the person
being interviewed, and precisely because of this I have chosen to retain
terminology from the original transcripts. Line numbers refer to those parts of
the transcripts presented here.
Presentation of text and general comments
Extract
I
An
interview by a young male student with a woman friend on the topic of formative
influences on occupational choice.
I I:
relax - this is nothing that will be judged
[interruption]
2 R: I'm
not looking at the questions
3 I: All
I want is to find out a few things concerning your family,
4 educational, friends - aspirations,
motivations etcetera
5 R: okay
6 I: and
I'll give you feedback on it - later on
7 R: not
now
8 I: no
- you can have anything you have said which you do not want
9 to be disclosed erased
10 R: aha
11 1: you
can also have anonymity - you can choose your pseudonym
12 if you want to
13 R: oh fine
14 I: as
long as it isn't (interviewer's name)
15 R: [laughs]
16 I: oh
yes I'll be taking a few notes which have nothing to do with
17 your answers during the interview -
is this okay
18 R: you'll
be taking notes - I see - okay
19 I no
[interruption]
20 R: so you're going to be watching me as well
21 I: I'll
record anything that's interesting - relevant
22 R: I'll
put my hands behind my back then...
23 R: ...
but that's life - as long as I learn from my mistakes - what
24 was the question again - sorry
25 I: it's
okay you've answered it
26 R: good
- stop looking at my legs [laughs]
27 I: erase
28 R: sorry
I just wanted to say that - sorry
29 I: Hmm
- let's continue - okay - and cover your legs...
It
seems that the prior friendship enables the respondent (R) to comment on the
prevailing rules structuring the interview by highlighting the assumptions of
control implicit within the interviewer's (I) framing of the interview: the
feedback is 'not now' (1.7) but later. While it is important not to minimise
the ways interviews can become opportunities for sexual harassment (see later),
it is also possible to read this exchange as R asserting in a mock threatening
way her power to evaluate and censor the interview material. She comments on
being scrutinised both verbally ('you'll be taking notes - I see - okay', 1.18)
and visually ('so you're going to be watching me as well', 1.20). Throughout
this extract there is an air of what I read as amicably sarcastic compliance
with clear suggestions of how provisional this may be ('aha', 1.10) and how the
resistance can enter into what is available to be recorded even before the
explicit rights of later erasure can be exercised ('I'll put my hands behind my
back then', 1.22). R's apparently wilful misinterpretation of I's attempts to
assert his position as interviewer and hers as respondent occurs even before
the critical moment of embarrassing I with 'stop looking at my legs' (1.26).
Significantly, this is immediately after R's indication that she had become so
absorbed in her narration that she had forgotten the interviewing context
(1.23-4), and so this can be read as a correlative disengagement. It is
possible to read the two times (1.8 and 1.19) I says 'no' more as dismay and
suppressed repudiation of R's resistance rather than a refusal of any specific
feature of it.
Extract 2
A
male interviewer of a male acquaintance on his political involvement with
animal rights groups. In contrast to the tense atmosphere of Extract 1, I read
this extract as a more reflective and collaborative exchange in which the
interview gravitates to become an interview about interviewing.
1 I: Do
you think it was a wise move for me to get into psychology?
2 R: I
thought you were supposed to be interviewing me
3 I: what's
the difference
4 R: I
don't know
5 I: do
you think the person being interviewed can get as much or
6 more out of the interview as the
interviewer
7 R: yes
of course, they wouldn't agree to be interviewed otherwise.
8 People enjoy being asked questions
about what they are interested
9 in
10 I: But
the police don't ask if you agree to be interviewed
11 R: No,
but you don't have to say anything
Extract 3
A
woman interviewer of a woman friend on the topic of the importance of
friendship.
1 P: .. I know this is difficult for you (I's name) but you did ask
me
2 to
3 do this and I couldn't possibly talk
about friendship without
4 talking about you
5 I: right
OK then (laughs)
6 P: Right,
it's very very important to me but erm it's quite difficult
7 actually because I'm talking to you
actually am I talking to you,
8 the person or you an interviewer?
9 I: I
think we can say you can talk about me as a person it can't be
10 objective it's subjective anyway
11 P: It's
really weird to be actually talking about you erm it s not
12 embarrassing is it? I have to do it.
13 I: a
bit but you will have to be clear for people reading it where we
14 met and that
15 P: I
can talk away from you if you want, right I will sort of Yea
16 the person I'm talking about her name
is...
17 I: do
you think it [the interview] could be improved?
18 P: no
I don't because I wouldn't be, if I had somebody, if the
19 interviewer was somebody who, a
person I didn't know, I really
20 don't think I would convey my feelings
with very many people
21 erm in actual fact I would only do
this interview for you I
22 wouldn't do it for anybody else I
certainly wouldn't talk about
23 friendship the topic or whatever with
anybody else it would
24 have to be a close friend
25 I: how
do you feel about all of this being seen by others?
26 P: How
do I feel about that, I don't mind really I don't mind
27 I: There
is so much material on there and I just feel I'm going to use
28 it for adverse means in a sense and I
wouldn't like it er
29 P: No
no I don't mind the reason I don't mind is that whatever is
30 recorded on there whatever is
transcribed on to paper will be in your
31 hands that's what's important to me
No it doesn't matter actually
32 whatever you come up with it really
doesn't matter. But
33 nevertheless it's in your hands
you're taking care of it it's your
34 baby I'm entrusting my feelings my
thoughts and feelings to you
35 which I already know I feel very
comfortable with anyway this makes
36 no difference in a sense it really
doesn't.
This
interview moves from an emphasis on the meaning of friendship in general to a
focus on support, and supportive friends in particular. The participant (P)
uses the interview to tell the interviewer (I) about the importance of I's
friendship to her; that is, she uses the new set of positions within their
relationship combined with the public nature of the encounter to comment on the
value of her relationship with I. The positions are explicitly marked where P
asks 'you the person or you an interviewer' (3.7-8), which sets up a
corresponding set of positions for her as both person and respondent. She
constructs I as a research object to include her as proper material for the
study by 'talking away from you' (3.15) and describing her in the third person
('the person I'm talking about her name is...', 3.16).
Having introduced the material, we can now
use four themes by which the extracts as a whole can be viewed in relation to
the analytic questions identified earlier. You might want to consider what your
choice of themes might be before reading further.
Theme 1
Using the position of respondent
In
Extract 1, R can be seen as asserting her power as respondent in a context in
which I is, in this case quite explicitly, dependent on R for the completion of
his work. In addition, the embarrassment ('erase', 1.27) over looking at, or
being positioned as looking at, R's legs highlights the covert sexuality, in
this case heterosexuality, of the research encounter. In this research
encounter, the official power of definition and interpretation coincides with
that of the active viewer and evaluator. These culturally masculine positions
were here held by a male researcher. R demonstrates her power over the
interpretation of the encounter by invoking the framework of harassment, thus
enlisting a range of judicial and disciplinary practices. One reading of this
exchange, then, is that by making the potential sexual exploitation of the
encounter explicit, the female research participant both comments on this
convergence of gender, sexual and interview power relations and resists being positioned as a
passive subject of the research gaze by using the structures of the research
(question-asking, recording) to turn the researcher himself into an object of
scrutiny.
In Extract 2 it is the interviewer who
sanctions the commentary about the interview process with 'do you think it was
a wise move for me to get into psychology?' (2.1). The explicit discussion of
interviewing roles follows from I's assertion of, and departure from, the role
of interviewer by asking a question, but of a reflexive form that transgresses,
transcends or reverses the current interviewing relationship.
In Extract 3 the additional positions made
available by the interview appear to be used by P to enable her to thank I and
express her appreciation of I as a friend. Using the rules of the interview,
not only is P required to hear this (owing to the injunction upon I to listen
to and respect what P says), but it is also justified within the rules of
interviewing, owing to the norm of frank disclosure conventionally assumed of
research participants.
Theme 2
Respondent-initiated reflexive commentaries
In
these extracts, although in different ways, the respondents initiate the
reflection on the research process. In Extract 1, R exploits the novice I's
highhandedness in reminding her of the research contract, which he appears to
inform her of, rather than negotiate. She therefore frustrates I's attempts to
assume the position of authority as the one who could but will not judge (1.1), who defines what is relevant to
record (1.21), who has defined the questions (1.3) to be answered, and who has
the authority to provide feedback (1.6). In Extract 2, R challenges I's assumption
that he can change the roles or rules of the interview with his 'I thought you
were supposed to be interviewing me' (2.2). In Extract 3, in an interview about
friendship, it is significant that P raises the reflexive issues about her
friendship with I. This emphasizes how commentaries on the research process are
by no means the prerogative of interviewers. Rather such cues and feedback are
always present even if unarticulated, and form the infrastructure of research.
Theme 3
Public nature of the account
It
is clear that the fact that these are interviews, rather than conversations,
constitutes key structural conditions for the accounts generated (and the
division between the 'public' and 'private' itself has a history). R's
resistance in Extract 1 to the interviewer-respondent relation is exercised
precisely through the public nature of their encounter. I's reaction to her
request to 'stop looking at my legs' is in terms of his awareness of the
tape-recording (he says 'erase', 1.27), while R reasserts her desire to record
this (literally reinscribes it) with 'sorry I just wanted to say that - sorry'
(1.28). R therefore enlists the broader structure of scrutiny hinted at earlier
by I's prohibition that R should use his name as a pseudonym (1.14), which
would place him as doubly subjected to the interview (as interviewer and
respondent). (In psychoanalytic terms we might see this as a return of the
repressed with a vengeance!)
Within
Extract 2, I initiates the discussion over who benefits from the research ('do
you think the person being interviewed can get as much or more out of the
interview as the interviewer', 2.5-6). I
also develops R's response in terms of people enjoying talking about what they
are interested in (2.8-9) by importing a legal-political interpretation of
people's 'interests' (in police interrogations) (2.10). The comparison between
legal/coercive regulation and the structure of the interview is both taken up
and refused by R in his reminder of participants' voluntary involvement as conditional
on personal engagement or relevance, as exhibited by the strategies available
for non-compliance within even the most formal structures of interrogation
(2.7-11). It may be appropriate that I defers to R over the success of this
strategy; the probability that he has had to exercise this is rendered more
likely by the topic of the interview. In Extract 3, the public nature of the
account as well as the topic (friendship) is a prerequisite for P expressing
and reasserting her trust in I. Both parties refer to a (temporally distanced
but very present) audience ('you will have to be clear for people reading it',
3.13, and 'how do you feel about all of this being seen by others', 3.25).
Theme 4 Past/future relations
In
all three extracts, the shifts in I-P positions are achieved by reference to,
and in relation to, their relationships outside the interviews. In 1, the teasing
tenor of the interview suggests that R wields her power through the prior
relationship she has with him. In 2, I marks permission to digress from the
previously agreed topic of the interview by asking R a question that invites
judgement of I on the basis of their prior relationship. He asks R for his
perception of the rules of the interview with 'what's the difference' (2.3). The
past relationship is alluded to by reference to the choice to be interviewed.
As interviewer 2 commented to me after reading this analysis, either his
mistrust of psychology is well-placed, or else, as a prior acquaintance, the
interviewer is a particularly clever policeman to trick him into thinking he
has a choice. In Extract 3, by the end of the interview the research encounter
is incorporated into the structure of the existing friendship as not only a
test of the relationship ('it's in your hands... I'm entrusting my feelings my
thoughts and feelings to you', 3.33-4), but as an expression of it ('I would
only do this interview for you', 3.22, 'this makes no difference in a sense it
really doesn't', 3.35-6), and by the end the interview itself is in the past,
as 'there' on the tape (not here and now), in 'there 15 50 much material on
there' (3.27), and as a record that can be used or abused.
Reflexive analysis
In
this section we locate the analysis within an account of its production,
including relevant constraints, limits and possibilities wrought by our
position as analysts. These are issues that might be regarded as the context of
the research, and this 'context' (in the sense of what accompanies and
constructs the text) can be divided into ten points.
Records - what's lost (and gained)
The
tendency to confound interview with transcript discussed earlier is in some
senses clarified by the fact that the analysis presented here is based on
transcripts of interviews which I did not conduct or transcribe. Paradoxically,
the fact that I did not conduct the interviews puts my position closer to that
of other readers in both generating and justifying the analysis. What this
brings home is the incompleteness and partiality of interpretation (see below),
qualities intrinsic to this kind of research but made more manifest in these
circumstances. I have reproduced the extracts using the notation adopted by the
original interviewers/transcribers. The use of conventional punctuation and
absence of explicit transcription codes in part reflected their concern about
presenting to informants a formalised and inaccessible representation of the
interview that might compound further the potentially alienating effect of
seeing spoken language written down in all its (in terms of written codes)
untidiness (Stubbs 1983). But these extracts would perhaps have gained from a
more systematic coding (see the Appendix for a simplified notation suitable for
most thematically based analyses).
Overinterpretation/misinterpretation
A
common reaction to analyses of the kind offered above is that the material has
been misinterpreted or overinterpreted, manipulated to produce meanings that
were not 'originally' there. It is certainly true that the process of analysis,
including the shifting representational forms that the interview/text assumes,
does provide new vantage points from which to interpret, and that, as Stubbs
(1983) points out, close scrutiny of a transcript can magnify tensions or
aggressive elements within the text. However, acknowledging this does not
discredit the analysis offered; rather it supports the suggestion that this is
one of multiple ways of reading the texts. Clearly if the reading is mine
alone, that is, if this reading is not recognised by others, then its credibility
is undermined. Similarly, the reactions of the original interviewers and
participants in the extracts are important. Depending on the model of research
adopted, disagreement would invalidate the reading (within an ethnographic
model) or be interpreted within it (since from a postmodernist stance it could
be argued that there may be particular investments in refusing the
interpretation; Opie 1992). Here it is worth saying that the original
interviewers' reactions to this analysis were very favourable, and no
reservations were expressed about the readings outlined above, nor any
alternatives put forward. In terms of additional points, the interviewer in
Extract 1 wished to have further emphasized how much is lost in the transition
from tape to transcript. Feedback from interviewer and interviewee 2 confirmed
the view of the interview process as an elaboration of mutual agendas, and
indicated a view that this should be recognised and explicitly structured into
interview procedures.
Partial interpretation
All
this emphasizes the constructed and inexhaustive nature of the analysis. These
features sometimes make this kind of research both frustrating and
dissatisfying, since a common response is to feel acutely the partiality (in
the double sense of incomplete and motivated) of interpretation. Nevertheless,
this too is an instructive reflection on the research process. It is helpful
here to explore the infinite regress and incoherence of the fantasy of a
complete and authentic original record (Why use audiotape, why not use
videotape? But what about that space behind the camera?), which parallels that
of the complete interpretation. Rather, we should accept the uncertainty of
unfinished analysis as an index of the arbitrary limit imposed by writing up. In
principle the research process could continue almost indefinitely, both in the
sense of exchanging readings and reactions between interpreter and
participants, and in the analyst's shifting perceptions of their
interpretations (and clearly some such mutual discussion and reflection with
participants is good research practice).
Intentionality and multiple readings
There
are clearly other ways of interpreting the extracts above. In fact it is often
useful to present and develop alternative readings and explore the different
conclusions they indicate. For my purposes I have selected one reading, but in
drawing attention to its provisionality two issues should be noted. First,
acknowledging the multiplicity of readings (or accounts of the account) does
not mean that all readings are equal. Otherwise we are stuck in the quagmire of
relativistic nihilism which disempowers us from using the research to say
anything (see Bhavnani 1990; Burman 1990). There may be good reasons for
privileging the reading or account of the research participants, particularly
if their 'voice' is that of a disadvantaged or under-represented group (but see
problems with this in Chapter 8). Second, and this is particularly relevant for
the kind of meta-analysis I have presented here, it is important not to equate
the reading with either the intentions of participants or their
intentions in the interview. The
analytic questions driving the analysis offered here were concerned with
research process and not individual opinions, so the focus was correspondingly
on implicit features of the rules or frameworks structuring research rather
than on what each party said or did. In this sense other analyses of this
material could be made in relation to different questions from those explored
here.
Selection of material
A
reflexive account could include examination of my own motives in presenting
this material. Perhaps I am aiming to demonstrate something about my teaching
practice through the interviewing competence of my students. Perhaps I am avoiding
subjecting my own interviewing and analytic practice to critical scrutiny (but
see Burman 1992a, b). A more standard criticism here would question the
validity of generating this analysis from interviews conducted on different
topics and by different interviewers. However, once again this approach can be
defended by recalling that the analytic issues do not require absolute
comparability of text (indeed such a notion is questionable, see Chapter 1),
and in fact may benefit from the variety of positions adopted.
Privileged access
In
more typical circumstances, the researcher has privileged knowledge both of the
participants and of the experience of conducting the interviews. In this case
the extra knowledge I bring to bear on this material is my acquaintance with
the interviewers, the history of having supervised (and marked) their
interviewing research, and therefore my access to the larger transcripts from
which the above extracts were selected. Again, these differences in position
from which the extracts can be read have to be acknowledged rather than erased,
and can perhaps be put forward either to explain differences in or to fix
interpretation (Burman 1993).
Exploitation
A
legitimate question that should always be posed (both in conducting and in
evaluating research) is whether the participants have been exploited, that is,
whether their psychological or material conditions worsened through their
involvement in the research. In this case, permission was gained from all
parties to use the material, and they were consulted over the interpretations
drawn. As discussed in the last three chapters of this book, issues of
exploitation go beyond notions of 'informed consent' to include the use made of
the research.
Effects of prior relationships
These
arise in the form of the interviewers' prior knowledge of their participants
from other than research contexts, and of my prior knowledge of the
interviewers from a teaching context. In the first case it seems likely that
this facilitated greater disclosure and reflexive commentary, as well as
constituting the preconditions for some of the themes identified in the
analysis. The impact of the second issue is difficult to evaluate but clearly
does figure; quite how helpful or otherwise this is will depend on how
persuasive you as readers find this account. We are not, however,
suggesting that people should only
interview their friends, but rather we want to high-light how the prior
relationship (of acquaintance, or non-acquaintance, which is also a relationship)
enters into the structure and content of the encounter.
Danger of fetishing particular strategies
Similarly,
the analysis here should not be read as recommending particular interviewing
devices in order to promote either equality or reflection on research
processes. Used as techniques these can work to assert interviewer authority in
indirect ways (see Walkerdine 1988: 53-63; Burman 1992a, for examples from
research with children). The examples presented here arose spontaneously, and
as initiated or at least taken up by the 'respondents'. The analysis here does
not (and cannot) illustrate how interviews should be done, but rather offers
suggestions about what to look for, and how to think about what happens in
interviews. There are no techniques or analytic procedures that escape the
dangers of exploitation. Hence it is important to structure consultation and
feedback over the interpretation of transcripts with participants.
Interpretive stance/countertransference
issues
Given
the nine points above, it is clear that the analyst brings to his or her
analysis a range of different identifications and responses. In these extracts
I identify as interviewer struggling to democratise the research process and
anxious about how to make sense of the material (perhaps anyone who has engaged
with semi-structured interviews can identify with the sense of responsibility
that the interviewer comments on in Extract 3, line 25). I also hold multiple
positionings and identifications arising from structures of gender, class and
so on, which inevitably enter into the particular analysis formulated. Where
personal reactions or investments play an important part, these can be treated
as a resource for, rather than 'interference' within, the analysis (for an
example of this see Marks, in press).
Discussion and assessment
The analysis presented here is based on
unusual material. These examples may not be everyday but they are also not
unique, and are consistent with the aim of highlighting the visibility of the researcher rather than the researched.
Qualitative analysis elucidates phenomena that would be missed or dismissed by
other methods. Just as the exception can prove the rule, so exceptional or
incidental instances can function to highlight structural dynamics that underlie
research encounters.
Part of what is so arresting about this
material, in particular Extract 1, is that it refers quite literally to what is
absent or lost from records of research: all too often we analyse interviews as
disembodied voices but interviews are interactions between embodied people. In
the example discussed here, R made this visible by protesting against I's
gaze. Issues of sexuality within research encounters are rarely commented upon
in accounts of research, but this example illustrates aspects that must always
be present, even if suppressed. Where the gender and researcher-researched
positions are reversed, that is where men are interviewed by women, issues of
participant resistance have been documented as being acted out in the form of
sexual harassment of researchers (see also McKee and O'Brian 1983; Reynolds
1993). It seems that where traditional power relations are departed from, where
men are positioned as subordinate within the researcher-researched relation,
for example, this structural power relationship becomes evident through their
attempts to subvert it. The disjunction or exception highlights the presence of
the rule.
We should draw attention to the concept of
power that has informed this analysis. Although it is concerned with instances
of 'respondent' resistance or assertion, this analysis does not imply that
'respondents' have greater power than 'interviewers'. Rather, following a
Foucauldian perspective, power is not conceived of as a unidimensional quality
that is possessed or lacked. This analysis has illustrated how particular
possibilities for assertion and resistance are produced through the structures
of the research encounter. Specifically, it has been argued that: (a) once
consent has been given, 'respondents' or 'participants' are not passive parties
to the question-answer interviewing structure; (b) they can assume a range of
strategies to resist that positioning; (c) they intervene in and comment on the
research process, as well as being the research focus; and (d) they achieve
both joint and separate goals through their participation in the research.
In evaluating the conduct and analysis of
interviews, there are continuities between thematic and textual approaches,
and similarities (although cast within rather different terminology and
corresponding philosophical basis) between the account here and 'grounded
theory' as elaborated by the criteria identified by Henwood and Pidgeon (1993:
24-7) of 'keeping close to the data; theory integrated at diverse levels of abstraction;
reflexivity; documentation; theoretical sampling and negative case analysis,
sensitivity to negotiated realities, transferability.' While most of the
limits of this approach have been addressed in the reflexive analysis, it
should be noted that interviewing is time-consuming, absorbing and suited to a
study with a restricted number of interviews in order to keep the transcription
and analysis of material manageable, and to do justice to the material
generated. In terms of problems, the multiple frameworks informing
semi-structured interviewing can make the approaches appear atheoretical or
intuitivist. A disciplined analysis of the presuppositions guiding all stages
of the research does much to ward off this criticism. The final analysis, however,
lies beyond the account here in the interpretations made of this by you.
Appendix
Suggested transcription notation:
(.) pause
(2) two
second pause (number indicates duration)
xxx untranscribable
(xxx) indistinct/doubtful
transcription
Word underline emphasis
Potter and Wetherell (1987: 188-9) suggest a slightly
more complex notation, which in turn is a simplified version of Sacks et at (1974).
Useful reading
Gubrium, J. and Silverman, D. (eds) (1989).
The Politics of Field Research. London:
Sage.
Henwood, K. and Pidgeon, N. (1993).
'Qualitative research and psychological theorising', in M. Hammersley (ed.) Social Researching: Philosophy, Politics and
Practice. London: Sage.
Nencel, L. and Pels, P. (eds) (1991). Constructing Knowledge: Authority and
Critique in Social Science. London: Sage.
Steier, F. (ed.) (1991). Research and Reflexivity. London: Sage.
References
Bhavnani, K.K. (1990). '"What's power
got to do with it?" Empowerment and social research', in I. Parker and J.
Shotter (eds) Deconstructing Social
Psychology. London: Routledge.
Bingham, W. and Moore, B. (1959). How to Interview. New York:
Harper International.
Burman, E. (1990). 'Differing with
deconstruction: a feminist critique', in I. Parker and J. Shotter (eds) Deconstructing Social Psychology. London:
Routledge.
Burman, E. (1992a). 'Feminism and discourse in
developmental psychology: identification, subjectivity and interpretation'. Feminism and Psychology, 2 (1), 45-60.
Burman, F. (1992b). 'Identification and power in
feminist therapy: a reflexive history of a discourse analysis'. Women's Studies International Forum, 15 (4), 487-98.
Burman,
F. (1993). 'Beyond discursive relativism: power and subjectivity in
developmental psychology', in H. Stam, L. Mos, W. Thorngate and B. Caplan (eds)
Recent Trends in Theoretical Psychology,
Volume III. New York: Springer Verlag.
Figueroa,
H. and Lopez, M.M. (1991). 'Commentary on Second Discourse Analysis
Workshop/Conference'. Paper presented at the Second Discourse Analysis
Workshop/Conference, Manchester, July.
Gubrium,
J. and Silverman, D. (eds) (1989). The
Politics of Field Research. London: Sage.
Henwood,
K. and Pidgeon, N. (1993). 'Qualitative research and psychological theorising',
in M. Hammersley (ed.) Social
Researching: Philosophy, Politics and Practice. London: Sage.
McKee, L. and O'Brian, M. (1983).
'Interviewing men: "taking gender seriously" ', in F. Garmarnikow, D.
Morgan, J. Purvis and D. Taylorson (eds) The
Public and the Private. London: Heinemann.
Marks, D. (forthcoming/in
press) 'Gendered care dynamics and the structuring of group relations:
child-professional-parent' in F. Burman, P. Alldred, C. Bewley, C. Heenan, D.
Marks, J. Marshall, K. Taylor, R. Ullah and S. Warner, Challenging Women: Psychology's Exclusions, Feminist Possibilities. Buckingham:
Open University Press.
Mishler,
F. (1986). Research Interviewing: Context
and Narrative. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Nencel,
L. and Pels, P. (eds) (1991). Constructing
Knowledge: Authority and Critique in Social Science. London: Sage.
Ochs,
E. (1979). 'Transcription as theory', in F. Ochs and B. Schieffelin (eds) Developmental Pragmatics. New York:
Academic Press.
Opie,
A. (1992). 'Qualitative research, appropriation of the "other" and
empowerment', Feminist Review, 40,
52-69.
Piaget,
J. (1929). The Child's Conception of the
World. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Potter,
J. and Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse
and Social Psychology. London: Sage. Reason, P. and Rowan, J. (eds) (1981).
Human Inquiry: a Sourcebook of New Paradigm
Research. Chichester: Wiley.
Reynolds,
G. (1993). '"And Gill came tumbling after": gender, emotions and a
research dilemma', in M. Kennedy, C. Lubelska and V. Walsh (eds) Making Connections: Women's Studies, Women's
Movements, Women's Lives. London:
Taylor and Francis.
Sacks,
H., Schegloff, F. and Jefferson, G. (1974). 'The simplest semantics for the
organisation of turn-taking in conversation', Language, 50, 697-737.
Spender,
D. (1981). 'The gatekeepers: a feminist critique of academic publishing', in H.
Roberts (ed.) Doing Feminist Research. London:
Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Spradley,
J. (1979). The Ethnographic Interview. New
York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Steier,
F. (ed.) (1991). Research and
Reflexivity. London: Sage.
Stubbs, M. (1983). Discourse
Analysis: the Sociolinguistic Analysis of Natural language. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Tedlock, D. (1984). The
Spoken Word and the Work of Interpretation. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania Press.
Walkerdine, V. (1988). The Mastery of Reason. London: Routledge.
Acknowledgements
I
am very grateful to the interviewers and participants whose talk and work I
draw upon in this chapter, in particular for their prompt and supportive
feedback on earlier drafts.