Is Self-Esteem
Political?
Sonja J. ELLIS[1]
Feminism and Psychology, (1998) vol. 8, no. 2, ps
251-256.
Self-esteem
is one of the most widely discussed constructs in psychological, educational
and in lay and popular discourse (e.g. Branden, 1987; Pottebaum et al., 1986;
Watkins et al., 1988). One of the main
reasons for this widespread interest in self-esteem,
is that it is believed to be fundamental to the social and psychological
well-being of individuals (Kohn, 1994).
However,
in examining the literature on self-esteem, it is apparent that the self esteem
construct and the way it has been operationalised are somewhat dubious. Much work on self-esteem seems to pathologise
marginalised individuals or groups as 'possessing' low self-esteem. The purpose of this article is to explore
some of the ways in which self-esteem is measured and applied and how it may
contribute to or reinforce the marginalisation of already subordinated groups.
SELF-ESTEEM IN
'SCIENTIFIC' RESEARCH
Although self-esteem has been defined in a variety of ways (see
Ellis, 1996), the term has typically been used to refer to an evaluative
judgement which individuals make of themselves and their abilities (e.g. see
Coopersmith, 1967). Thus in
'scientific' research, self-esteem is presumed to be internal, stable and
global.
In
feminist writing, the concept of self-esteem has been used as a way of
describing the marks of oppression experienced by
women. For example, a number of writers
have highlighted the way in which many women have negative feelings about
themselves (e.g. see Daniluk, 1993).
Through self-help psychology we have been encouraged to enhance our
self-esteem in turn allowing us to feel good about ourselves, our bodies, and
our achievements (e.g. see Loulan, 1987; Squire, 1994). So, in feminist work, self-esteem has been
employed as a means of empowering women, in a world which positions us as
inferior to men. However, in contrast to
this positive use of self-esteem, scientific research has tended to
conceptualise and operationalise self-esteem in ways which in fact disempower
us.
In
psychological research, it has generally been presumed that self-esteem can be
defined and reliably measured.
Psychometric scales (e.g. the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory,
Coopersmith, 1967) are by far the most widely employed measures of self-esteem,
and have typically been used to establish whether self-esteem is correlated
with sex/gender, culture/ethnicity, class, social factors (e.g. unemployment,
suicide, etc.), personality, family and/or psychopathic variables. Although there is little consistency in the
findings reported in these studies, it is notable that there is (still) a
tendency to conclude that women (e.g. see O'Brien, 1991; Orr and Dinur, 1995;
Oyefeso and Zacheaus, 1990; Richman et al., 1985), and ethnic/cultural
minorities (e.g. see Halpin et al., 1981; Sethi and Calhoun, 1986; Verkuyten,
1990) have lower self-esteem than men and the white majority. Although it may be true that (some) women and
ethnic/cultural minorities do experience low self-esteem, the content and
application of measures employed by researchers tend to be culturally biased
and gendered. This bias and
marginalisation in psychometric measurement has already been highlighted by a
number of feminist writers (e.g. see Bem, 1993).
In
relation to self-esteem, a number of studies have shown that in non-western
settings, self-esteem is composed of and influenced by quite different
components to those suggested by conventional western measures. For example, a study by Watkins (1988) found
that although family, friends and school were important aspects in determining
the self-esteem of Filipino young adolescents, factors such as food, money and
clothes were also relatively important but are ignored by western instruments
(see also Watkins and Regmi, 1993, for a similar study of Nepalese children).
Similarly,
it is conceivable that if the self-esteem of adolescents in western societies
is contingent on physical appearance and social acceptance as recent research
suggests (see Harter, 1990), access to money and clothes may also contribute to
the self-esteem of western adolescents. Thus, it is possible that these scales ask
about things which are not pertinent (or at least of limited relevance) to the
self-esteem of target groups (see Ellis, 1996; Watkins et al., 1988), raising
questions as to the appropriateness of western models and measures for
assessing the self-esteem of both western and non-western adolescents.
As
well as being inherently ethnocentric in content, psychometric scales and other
structured measures discriminate against marginalised groups in the way they
elicit responses. For example, in Maori
culture[2] a high value
is placed on whakaiti (being humble,
or denigrating oneself) rather than on whakahihi
(boasting, or putting oneself forward) (Ranby, 1979), yet psychometric
measures rely on individuals to do the latter.
A similar case may also be argued with regard to gender differences in
response patterns.
Despite
knowing this, researchers have continued to measure self-esteem in the ways
that they always have, without questioning the appropriateness or otherwise of
their instruments. Furthermore,
researchers have assumed that self-esteem is internal, stable (unless some type
of intervention has been applied to modify it) and global, and that this
conceptualisation of self-esteem is universal (see Juhasz, 1985; Ranby, 1979).
A further
problem arises from the way in which 'scientific' research has been seen as
'objective', and that because measures have 'proven' validity and reliability,
they are presumed to provide (eternal) 'truths' about phenomena (Gergen,
1985). In psychological literature,
self-esteem has been talked about as if it were a permanent characteristic of
an individual, and that individuals therefore possess certain levels of self-esteem. However, the term 'self-esteem' is in fact a
social artefact created to meaningfully describe the way in which some
individuals feel good about themselves and others do not, but the terms 'low'
and 'high' self esteem have been treated as naturally occurring categories (see
Kitzinger, 1992).
So,
instead of talking about self-esteem as a phenomenon which might be transient
or subject to change, it has been conceptualised as something which is necessarily
a characteristic of particular individuals or groups. Thus the methods employed in research have
tended to manufacture individual/group
differences in self-esteem. Moreover,
researchers continue to ignore the possibility that their instruments are
socially, historically and culturally constructed (Gergen, 1985), and that they
might privilege the values of some groups over those of other groups, resulting
in the (needless) pathologisation of individuals from marginalised groups.
SELF-ESTEEM IN
APPLIED SETTINGS
Although
some individuals refer to themselves as 'having' low self-esteem, low
self-esteem does not actually exist within individuals, yet teachers and
counsellors presume to be able to 'recognise' self-esteem (or more commonly a
lack of it) in their students or clients (Dewhurst, 1991). Without reference to testing or measurement,
teachers and counsellors typically 'assess' an individual's self-esteem on a
basis of overt characteristics (e.g. family background, culture) and behaviours
or traits (e.g. shyness, social skills).
Again, more often than not, individuals from lower socioeconomic
families, cultural/ethnic 'minorities' and women are identified as having low
self-esteem, and therefore are perceived as deficient.
Moreover,
such allegations are extended to pathologising whole groups of individuals,
without considering the possibility that these groups may actually
conceptualise self-esteem differently from researchers and practitioners. Thus, the label of 'low self-esteem' has come
to be applied to (certain) groups of
individuals, assuming homogeneity among members of those groups. Consequently, the inequalities of power which
researchers and practitioners claim to be addressing are in fact being
maintained.
As a
result of the perceived connection between self-esteem and societal/educational
'problems' (see Barwick, 1992; Branden, 1987; Youth Mental Health Foundation,
1988), low self-esteem has been viewed as characteristic of 'at risk'
individuals, particularly adolescents. Because
marginalised groups are disproportionately represented in statistics relating
to truancy, delinquency, suicide, abuse and under achievement, and are
therefore assumed to be 'at risk', low self-esteem tends to be automatically
associated with marginalised ethnic/cultural groups, the unemployed, and those
from lower socioeconomic groups. The term ,self-esteem' is then bandied around as if it were
unproblematic.
Furthermore,
those identified as low in self-esteem are often encouraged to participate in
special programmes aimed at improving their self-esteem (usually to enhance
achievement or employability). For
example, currently in New Zealand, 'at risk' adolescents (usually Maori) are
selected for outdoor education programmes aimed at building confidence and
self-esteem. On completion of these
courses, participants are returned to the environments which were initially
identified as putting them 'at risk', and expected to maintain their enhanced
level of self-esteem.
It
would also appear that if the individual fails to achieve the desirable level
of self-esteem at the end of the programme, rather than perceiving the
'problem' as societal, or questioning the programme itself, it is typically the
already marginalised individual who is blamed.
Often, the prescribed solution is to send them on yet another course to
help them improve their self-esteem. For
example, after being unemployed for 12 months, it is compulsory for New
Zealanders to participate in 'training' courses (including one on self-esteem). Those who still fail to gain employment after
completing the courses are sent on further compulsory courses. The effect, then, of identifying individuals
as low in self-esteem may be to keep them on a self-esteem-seeking treadmill,
and to divert attention from the I real' social and
political issues.
SO, IS
SELF-ESTEEM POLITICAL THEN?
Is it
possible, then, that self-esteem is being used to maintain marginalisation, by
creating group differences which uphold inequalities?
It
would certainly seem that the way in which self-esteem has been 'constructed'
in psychological and lay discourse, limits the way(s) in which the self may be
thought about, because of the differential access afforded to individuals by
the discourses used. So, while it is
possible for certain individuals to perceive themselves
as 'successful' or 'intelligent', it may not be possible for others because of
the power structures inherent in the methodologies employed by psychological
researchers and practitioners.
Consequently, while individuals from marginalised groups may be experiencing 'low self-esteem',
the way in which this construct has been conceptualised and operationalised
upholds the patriarchal relations which position women as inferior to (and
dependent on) men, and subordinates women of colour.
Aside
from the argument of whether it is possible to measure self-esteem (a major
consideration which I have not discussed in this article), researchers and
practitioners need to consider whether it is appropriate to continue measuring,
assessing and even focusing on self-esteem.
Given the value-laden nature of the self-esteem construct, it would seem
that we need to focus our attention on finding alternative ways of describing
the 'marks of oppression' experienced by (certain) women. Furthermore, if self-esteem is so essential
to the well-being of individuals, there must be better ways of investigating
and dealing with this ,problem' than the somewhat
dubious and discriminatory measures and practices currently in use.
REFERENCES
Barwick, H. (1992) Youth
Suicide Prevention Project: Workshop and Literature Review. Wellington, New
Zealand: Department of Health.
Bem, S.L. (1993) The Lenses of Gender: Transforming the Debate on Sexual Inequality. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Branden, N. (1987) How
to Raise Your Self-Esteem. Toronto: Bantam
Books.
Coopersmith, S. (1967) The Antecedents of Self-Esteem. San
Francisco: W.H. Freeman.
Daniluk, J.C. (1993) 'The Meaning and Experience of
Female Sexuality', Psychology of Women
Quarterly 17: 53-69.
Dewhurst, D.W. (1991) 'Should Teachers Enhance Their
Pupils' Self-Esteem?', Journal of Moral Education 20(1): 3-11.
Ellis, S.J. (1996) 'What is Self-Esteem Anyway?: Fourth Formers' Perspectives on Self-Esteem', unpublished
Master's thesis, University of Waikato, New Zealand.
Gergen, K.J. (1985) 'Social Constructionist Inquiry:
Context and Implications', in K.J. Gergen
and K.E. Davis (eds) The Social
Construction ()f the Person. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Halpin, G., Halpin, G. and Whiddon, T. (1981) 'Locus
of Control and Self-Esteem among American Indians and Whites: A Cross-Cultural
Comparison', Psychological Reports 48:
91-8.
Harter, S. (1990) 'Self and
Identity Development', in S.S. Feldman and G.R. Elliott (eds) At the Threshold: The Developing
Adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Juhasz, A.M. (1985) 'Measuring Self-Esteem in Early
Adolescents', Adolescence 20: 877-87.
Kitzinger, C. (1992) 'The Individuated Self-Concept: A
Critical Analysis of Social Constructionist Writing on Individualism', in G.
Breakwell (ed.) Social Psychology of
Identity- and the self-concept. Guildford: Surrey University Press.
Kohn, A. (1994) 'The Truth about Self-Esteem', Phi Delta Kappan 76: 272-83.
Loulan, J. (1987)
Lesbian Passion. Duluth, MN: Spinsters Ink.
O'Brien, E.J. (1991) 'Sex Differences in Components of
Self-Esteem', Psychological Reports 68:
241-2.
Orr, E. and Dinur, B. (1 995) 'Social Setting Effects
on Gender Differences in Self-Esteem:
Kibbutz and Urban Adolescents', Journal of Youth and Adolescence 24: 3-27.
Oyefeso, A.0. and Zacheaus,
A. (1990) 'Self-Esteem as Determined by Gender Differences Among Yoruba
Adolescents in Ibadan, Nigeria: A Research Note', Journal of Child Psychology, Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 331:461-3.
Pottebaum, S.M., Keith, T.Z. and Ehly, S.W. (1 986)
'Is There a Causal Relation between Self-Concept and Academic Achievement?', Journal of
Educational Research 79: 140-4.
Ranby, P. (1979) 'What Do Maori Children Think of
Themselves?', Set
1(10).
Richman, C.L., Clark, M.L. and Brown, K.P. (1985)
'General and Specific Self-Esteem in Late Adolescent Students: Race x Gender x
SES Effects', Adolescence 20: 555-66.
Sethi, R.R. and Calhoun, G. (1986) 'Comparison of
Total Self-Esteem Scores on Coopersmith's Inventory for Pupils from India and
the United States', Psychological Reports
59: 523-6.
Squire, C. (1994) 'Empowering Women? The Oprah
Winfrey Show', Feminism & Psychology 4(1):
63-79.
Verkuyten, M. (1990) 'Self-Esteem and the Evaluation
of Ethnic Identity among Turkish and Dutch Adolescents in the Netherlands', Journal of Social Psychology 130:
285-97.
Watkins, D. (1988) 'Components of Self-Esteem of
Children from a Deprived Cross-Cultural Background', Social Behavior and Personality 16 (1): 1-3.
Watkins, D., Alabaster, M. and Freemantle, S. (1988)
'Assessing the Self-Esteem of New Zealand Adolescents', New Zealand Journal of Psychology 17: 32-5.
Watkins, D. and Regmi, M. (1993) 'The Basis of
Self-Esteem of Urban and Rural Nepalese Children', Journal of Social Psychology 133: 255-7.
Youth Mental Health Foundation (1988) Under Pressure. New
Zealand: Department of Health/Te Tari Ora.
Sonja J. ELLIS is now a doctoral student in the
Department of Social Sciences at Loughborough University, Loughborough, LEI I
3TU, UK.
[email:S.J.Ellis@lboro.ac.uk]
[1] This article is based on issues raised in my Master's
thesis. I therefore wish to record my
sincere thanks to my supervisors, Dr Monica Payne and Dr Jenny Young-Loveridge,
whose input contributed to the development of the ideas presented in this
article. I also wish to thank Celia
Kitzinger, Margaret Wetherell and an anonymous reviewers.
for their helpful comments for revising the initial
version of this article.
[2] Maori are the indigenous peoples of Aotearoa/New
Zealand.