Multiple Identity Enactments and Multiple Personality Disorder: A Socio-cognitive Perspective
Nicholas ‘I’m just a
naughty little boy’ Spanos
Psychological Bulletin,
1994, vol. 116, no. 1, ps 143-165.
Abstract
People who enact multiple identities behave as if they possess 2 or more selves, each with its own characteristic moods, memories, and behavioural repertoire. Under different names, this phenomenon occurs in many cultures; in North American culture, it is frequently labelled multiple personality disorder (MPD). This article reviews experimental, cross-cultural, historical, and clinical findings concerning multiplicity and examines the implications of these findings for an understanding of MPD. Multiplicity is viewed from a socio-cognitive perspective, and it is concluded that MPD, like other forms of multiplicity, is socially constructed. It is context bounded, goal-directed, social behavior geared to the expectations of significant others, and its characteristics have changed over time to meet changing expectations.
Introduction
People
who receive the diagnosis of multiple personality disorder (MPD) behave as if
they possess two or more distinct identities.
They convey the impression of multiplicity by exhibiting a relatively
integrated interpersonal style (i.e., a distinct personality) when calling
themselves by one name and different interpersonal styles when calling
themselves by other names. Frequently,
MPD patients behave as if their different identities have their own unique
memories and experiences, and many of the identities claim amnesia for the
other personalities with whom they co-reside.
Modern
MPD patients are usually women with a wide range of chronic psychiatric
problems that predate their MPD diagnosis (Coons, Bowman, & Milstein, 1988;
Putnam, Guroff, Silberman, Barban, & Post, 1986; Ross, Norton, &
Wozney, 1989). These patients usually
claim to have been physically or sexually abused-often horrendously-in
childhood (Coons & Milstein, 1986; Ross, Miller, Bjornson, Reagor, Fraser,
& Anderson, 199 1; Young, Sachs, Braun, & Watkins, 199 1). Moreover, it is now common for investigators
(e.g., Bliss, 1986; Braun, 1990; Kluft, 1993; Putnam, 1989, 1993; Ross, 1989)
to argue that MPD is a distinct mental disorder caused by severe childhood
abuse. According to this hypothesis,
severe trauma during childhood produces a mental splitting or dissociation as a
defensive reaction to the trauma. These
dissociated "parts" of the person develop into alter identities or
personalities that, in adulthood, periodically manifest themselves to help the
individual cope with stressful situations, express resentments or other
feelings that the primary personality disavows, and so on. The proponents of this perspective have been
highly vocal, and, despite much and varied criticism, this view has become
highly influential. Thus, from this
perspective, displays of multiple identities reflect a mental disorder that
"happens" to the person as a result of early traumas and other
experiences over which she or he has no control and often no memory rather than
something that the person does in response to current contingencies, goals, and
understandings.
In
this article, I argue that the disease perspective of MPD is fundamentally
flawed. Specifically, I use
experimental, cross-cultural, and historical findings to argue that (a)
multiple identities are usefully conceptualised as rule-governed social
constructions, (b) neither childhood trauma nor a history of severe
psychopathology is necessary for the development or maintenance of multiple
identities, and (c) multiple identities are established, legitimated,
maintained, and altered through social interaction.
This
socio-cognitive alternative to the disease model suggests that MPD is a
socio-historical product (Hacking, 1986, 1992; Kenny, 1986; Shorter, 1992;
Spanos, 1989). In the last 2 centuries,
a number of psychiatric syndromes (e.g., motor hysteria) have developed,
spread, and then all but disappeared as a function of changing conceptions held
by both doctors and patients concerning the ways in which distress may be
legitimately expressed (Shorter, 1992).
In the last 20 years, the notion of multiple personality has become
commonplace in North American culture and is now a legitimate way for people to
understand and express their failures and frustrations, as well as a covert
tactic by which they can manipulate others and attain succour and other
rewards. In short, the socio-cognitive
perspective suggests that patients learn to construe themselves as possessing
multiple selves, learn to present themselves in terms of this construal, and
learn to reorganise and elaborate on their personal biography so as to make it
congruent with their understanding of what it means to be a multiple. These patients are conceptualised as actively
involved in using available information to create a social impression that is
congruent with their perception of situational demands and with the
interpersonal goals they are attempting to achieve (Spanos, Weekes, &
Bertrand, 1985).
According
to this perspective, psychotherapists play a particularly important part in the
generation and maintenance of MPD.
Therapists routinely encourage patients to construe themselves as having
multiple selves, provide them with information about how to convincingly enact
the role of "multiple personality patient," and provide official
legitimation for the different identities that their patients enact.
Identities: Singular and Plural
In
all cultures, people exhibit wide variability in their behavior across time and
situations. Nevertheless, in North
American culture almost all people hold a subjective sense of unitary identity
and view their diverse behavior as the product of a single self (Epstein, 1973). The self is a social product, a series of
interrelated construals made about the first person singular by the first
person singular (Deaux, 1993). These
self-construals reflect the categories that each culture uses to describe its
members and involve socially derived attributions that people apply to
themselves. The way that people view
themselves reflects the way that they are viewed by others, and it is others
who provide or withhold legitimation for the varied self-presentations that
people enact (Goffman, 1959). People
typically present themselves as a unitary self enacting different roles because
they are reinforced for doing so. In
fact, the social, economic, and legal institutions of North American culture
are premised on the notion that each person is a unitary self who is accountable
for his or her own diverse behaviors, and it is this view of unitary self that
is routinely legitimated in social interaction (Halleck, 1990; Mancuso &
Sarbin, 1983; M. S. Moore, 1984).
Social
products are, of course, subject to social change. There is nothing invariable or inevitable
about the notion of a unitary self, and the same social processes that validate
the conception people hold of themselves as unitary selves can be used to
validate the alternative conceptualisation that people consist of multiple
selves.
Hypnosis and Multiple Personality Disorder
In
North American culture, those who carry out multiple identity enactments are
usually defined by mental health professionals as suffering from MPD. Historically, MPD has been closely tied to
hypnotic phenomena. Some modern
investigators (e.g., Bliss, 1986; Braun, 1990) have argued that in predisposed
children, trauma produces a "hypnotic state" that facilitates the
development of alter personalities.
Purportedly, these alters remain separated from normal consciousness by
a process akin to hypnotic amnesia. In
addition, modem experiments aimed at elucidating the nature of multiplicity
have been conducted within a hypnotic context.
Consequently, background information concerning hypnotic responding in
general and hypnotic amnesia in particular may be useful in understanding MPD
enactments.
Hypnotic Responding
Historically,
hypnosis has been viewed as an altered state of consciousness that greatly
increases responsiveness to suggestions and that, in highly hypnotisable
subjects, produces distortions in memory and perception and facilitates the
recall of "hidden" memories.
This view of hypnosis is almost always uncritically accepted in the MPD
literature (e.g., Bliss, 1986; Ross, 1989).
In the last 40 years, however, a great deal of empirical evidence has
challenged this view (for reviews, see Spanos, 1986b; Spanos & Chaves,
1989; Wagstaff, 1981). Contrary to the
assumption frequently found in the MPD literature, more than a century of
research has failed to uncover unambiguous behavioural, physiological, or
subjective report criteria for denoting a uniquely hypnotic state (Barber,
1979; Fellows, 1986; Radtke & Spanos, 1981; Sarbin & Coe, 1972). In addition, a large number of studies:
indicate that even the seeming dramatic behaviors associated with high
hypnotizability (e.g., displays of age regression and amnesia) can be
accomplished by motivated control subjects who have not been administered
hypnotic induction procedures and who display no signs of being in a
"trancelike" condition (Barber, 1969; Diamond, 1974; Wagstaff,
1981). Furthermore, many of these
dramatic behaviors have turned out to be much more mundane than they originally
appeared. For example, hypnotically
age-regressed subjects do not develop the psychological characteristics of
actual children; instead, they respond like adults attempting to behave as if
they are children. When their
expectations of how children behave are inaccurate, their age regression
performances are off the mark (Nash, 1987).
MPD
theorists frequently imply that hypnosis is a process that happens to a person
rather than something that a person does (Bliss, 1986). However, there is much support for the
hypothesis that hypnotic behaviors are goal-directed enactments and that highly
hypnotisable subjects are cognizing individuals who are attuned to even subtle
interpersonal cues and who are invested in meeting the social demands of
hypnotic situations to present themselves as "good" subjects (Sarbin
& Coe, 1972; Spanos & Coe, 1992).
The demands contained in hypnotic suggestions call for particular
subjective experiences as well as corresponding overt behaviors. For instance, suggestions for amnesia require
not only that subjects fail to report target material but also that they define
themselves as having forgotten that material.
However, suggested experiences such as temporary forgetting or reduced
pain do not occur automatically.
Instead, such experiences must be generated by subjects who use their
attentional and imaginal abilities in attempting to create these subjective
effects. Subjects who are unable to
generate the subjective experiences called for by suggestions frequently admit
their failures rather than fake their responses. On the other hand, hypnotic responding is
exceedingly easy to fake (Orne, 1979), some subjects do purposely describe
their experiences inaccurately to meet test demands, and such erroneous
descriptions are most likely among highly hypnotisable subjects given difficult
suggestions (Burgess, Spanos, Ritt, Hordy, & Brooks, 1993; Spanos, Flynn,
& Gabora, 1989; Wagstaff, 198 1).
Hypnotic Amnesia
Amnesia
has long been a hallmark of hypnotic responding, and during the 19th century it
was thought to occur spontaneously as a function of the transition from being
hypnotised to being awake (Sarbin & Coe, 1972). MPD patients frequently behave as if one or
more of their alter identities are amnesic for the memories of other alters. MPD theorists typically describe such amnesia
as an involuntary and spontaneous occurrence that is akin to hypnotic amnesia
(Bliss, 1986; Ross, 1989). Contrary to
such descriptions, however, amnesic displays are not a common accompaniment of
hypnotic performances unless the amnesia has been suggested explicitly (Coe,
1989). Spontaneous amnesia is a rare
occurrence and, when it does occur, may simply reflect subjects' implicit
understandings of the hypnotic role. Even
highly hypnotisable subjects typically recall the contents of their hypnotic
session after its termination unless they are explicitly suggested to do
otherwise.
When
asked to describe the experience of hypnotic amnesia, subjects proffer a wide
range of reports. Contrary to the way in
which hypnotic amnesia has been described in the MPD literature (e.g., Bliss,
1986), many subjects describe their forgetting as an active process that
involves self-distraction and other strategies aimed at inhibiting recall
(Spanos & Bodorik, 1977). Some
hypnotic subjects do report that they were unable to recall target material and
that they perceived their amnesia as involuntary. Nevertheless, the available experimental data
indicate that even these subjects retain rather than lose control of memory
processes and guide their recall in terms of the social demands to which they
are exposed (Coe, 1989; Spanos & Coe, 1992). For example, in two separate studies (Silva
& Kirsch, 1987; Spanos, Radtke, & Bertrand, 1984), highly hypnotisable
hypnotic subjects who were displaying amnesia were convinced that they would be
able to recall the forgotten information but only under certain
conditions. All of the subjects in one
study and almost all in the other behaved in terms of the expectations conveyed
to them by recalling and failing to recall forgotten material in the
appropriate sequences. Relatedly, Coe
and Sluis (1989) exposed highly hypnotisable subjects who exhibited
posthypnotic amnesia to strong and repeated demands to remember. Under these circumstances, even subjects who
had insisted that their amnesia was involuntary showed substantial recovery of
the forgotten memories.
MPD
patients are frequently described as living for years with alter personalities
of which they are unaware (Kluft, 1985).
However, hypnotic amnesia is rarely complete even among highly
hypnotisable subjects. Moreover, among
those few subjects who exhibit total amnesia, more than half report
post-experimentally that during the amnesia test period, they consciously
remembered but failed to report at least some of the target information (Spanos
& Bodorik, 1977).
The
"memory deficits" displayed by hypnotically amnesic subjects vary
dramatically as a function of the expectations to which they are exposed. Thus, depending on the suggestions they are
given, these subjects behave as if they have forgotten an entire list or only a
subset of the list; all of the concrete words on a list but none of the
abstract words, and vice versa; the number 4 while recalling all remaining
numbers; and so on (Coe, 1989).
Exhibiting the wide range of memory performance changes called for in
these different experiments requires that hypnotic subjects retain rather than
lose control of memory processes and guide their amnesic displays in terms of
their understanding of what is called for by the amnesia test situation.
MPD
patients also exhibit a wide range of amnesic deficits that are difficult to
explain in terms of involuntary memory dysfunction. Some report a one-way amnesia between alter
identities, whereas others report a two-way amnesia. When switching alter identities, most such
patients retain basic skills such as reading and writing. In some early cases, however, these abilities
were purportedly lost and had to be relearned by the new identity (Hacking,
1991; Kenny, 1986). In many modern
cases, MPD patients switch back and forth between alters (and thereby between
sets of supposedly segregated memories) very quickly. However, in a series of 19th-century British
cases described by Hacking (1991), these kinds of switches between alters frequently
involved a period of transitional sleep.
Nineteenth-century and early-20th-century MPD patients rarely displayed
more than two or three alter identities (Bowman, 1990). Modern patients, on the other hand, display
an average of 15 or more alters, and some of these patients exhibit more than
100 alters (Ross, Norton, & Fraser, 1989).
In other words, the number of dissociated memory systems supposedly
possessed by MPD patients has grown exponentially since the beginning of this
century. These kinds of differences in
amnesic displays over time and across patients suggest that MPD patients, like
hypnotically amnesic subjects, alter their patterns of recall as a function of
their understanding of what is expected.
As the expectations of therapists concerning the amnesia of their
patients change, patients change their amnesic displays to meet the new
expectations.
Work
with non-MPD psychiatric patients also indicates that psychogenic amnesia is a
goal-directed achievement influenced by subjects' understandings and by
legitimation received from others rather than an involuntary occurrence
(Kirshner, 1973). For instance, Parfitt
and Gall (1944) worked with combat veterans whose reports of amnesia prevented
their return to active service. Rather
than legitimating these amnesic displays, Parfitt and Gall informed the
patients that their memory would return and continued to convey this
expectation in their interactions with the patients. Parfitt and Gall did not use any treatment to
lift patients' amnesia other than telling them that they could remember. Exposure to these consistent demands to
recall rather than to continue forgetting led most subjects to recover their
memories. On the basis of these and
other clinical findings, Kirshner (1973) suggested that psychogenic amnesia can
be construed as a transitional social role that is adopted to deal with
conflict and stress. Such an analysis
may be useful for explaining how hypnotic interventions that legitimate remembering
can often induce a "sharing" of memories among the several identities
enacted by multiples (R. B. Allison
& Schwarz, 1980; Bliss, 1980; Ross, 1989).
The available evidence indicates that hypnotic procedures do not possess
intrinsic properties that enhance recall (Smith, 1983; Wagstaff, 1989). However, such interventions may provide a
legitimating context for redefining the situation as one in which displays of
cross-identity remembering are considered role appropriate.
In
summary, MPD theorists may well be correct when they contend that the amnesia
of MPD patients resembles hypnotic amnesia.
However, the descriptions of hypnotic amnesia given by these theorists
are often misleading (e.g., Bliss, 1980, 1986).
Contrary to these descriptions, the available data indicate that
hypnotic amnesia does not involve an automatic and complete forgetting of
events. Instead, hypnotic amnesia (and
most likely much of MPD amnesia as well) involves goal-directed enactment aimed
at meeting social expectations. Such
amnesia is frequently defined by subjects as involving voluntary
self-distraction, it is rarely complete even in highly hypnotisable subjects,
and, when it is complete, it frequently involves conscious withholding and is
typically of short duration. Hypnotic
subjects retain rather than lose control of memory processes and, in that way,
display the particular memory deficits called for by the test situation (Coe,
1989; Spanos, 1986b).
Braun
(1990) suggested that the segregated memories displayed by different alters
reflect state-specific recall.
Application of this idea to the amnesia seen in MPD patients suggests
that experiences acquired in one psychological state (i.e., Identity A) will
not be remembered when the person is in a different state (i.e., Identity
B). However, these experiences will be
easily remembered when the person is again in the first state (i.e., Identity
A). This formulation cannot account for
the one-way amnesia that is commonly reported by MPD patients (e.g., Identity A
is aware of its own memories and those of Identity B, but Identity B is aware
only of its own memories). In addition,
this hypothesis cannot explain cases in which people who display distinct
identities show no cross-identity amnesia.
Heterosexual
male transvestites in North American culture alternate between a male and
female identity. When enacting their
female identity, they typically dress as women, act as women, and refer to
themselves with a woman's name (Talamini, 1982). Nevertheless, transvestites do not display
between-identities amnesia. While
enacting their female self, they recall and discuss their masculine self, and
vice versa (Docter, 1988). These
findings indicate that amnesia between identities does not arise automatically
as a function of the differences in psychological functioning that are involved
in enacting one identity as opposed to another.
Instead, whether or not amnesia is associated with alter identity
enactments appears to depend on the expectations and purposes associated with
those enactments.
Experimental Creation of
Multiplicity
In
the last 20 years, two lines of experimental research have examined variables
that influence the development of multiplicity.
The first was initiated by Hilgard (1979) and revolved around his notion
of a hidden observer or hidden self. The
second research line has dealt with the phenomenon of past-life hypnotic
regression.
Hidden Observer Experiments
Hilgard
(1979, 1991) conducted a series of studies that led to the elicitation of
"hidden selves" in highly hypnotisable college students. In a typical experiment, subjects were
exposed to Pain stimulation trials before and after administration of a
hypnotic procedure and suggestions for analgesia. Subjects were also given instructions that
implied that a hidden part of them remained aware of all they experienced
during the analgesia period. During
hypnotic analgesia testing, these subjects gave two types of pain reports. Verbal (overt) reports purportedly from their
hypnotised self usually indicated relatively low levels of pain, and covert
reports (numbers tapped out in a previously taught keypressing code) supposedly
from their hidden self usually indicated high levels of pain.
Hilgard
(1979, 1991) argued that the hidden self instructions used in these studies did
not provide subjects with the idea that they had a hidden self or with the idea
that hidden and overt reports should be different. Instead, the hidden self was supposedly an
unsuggested accompaniment of hypnotic analgesia that remained separated from
normal consciousness by an "amnesic barrier" unless and until it was
contacted by the hypnotist. Contrary to
this view, a number of studies (Spanos, Flynn, & Gwynn, 1988; Spanos,
Gwynn, & Stam, 1983; Spanos & Hewitt, 1980) demonstrated that the
direction of hidden reports varied with the expectations conveyed by hidden
self instructions. Thus, depending on
the implications contained in their instructions, subjects exhibited hidden
selves that reported less pain than, more pain than, or an amount of pain equal
to that of their hypnotised selves.
The
creation of hidden selves that respond to instruction by behaving as if they
possess information of which the person's "normal self " is unaware
has been documented in studies on hypnotic age regression, blindness, eye-witness
recall, and amnesia as well as pain reduction (Spanos et al., 1988; Spanos,
Gwynn, Corner, Baltruweit, & deGroh, 1989; Spanos & Mclain, 1986;
Spanos et al., 1984). Moreover, hypnotic
procedures are not required to produce hidden selves. Two studies (Spanos & Bures, 1993;
Spanos, deGroot, Tiller, Weekes, & Bertrand, 1985) found that nonhypnotic
high hypnotizables reported experiencing hidden selves as frequently as did
corresponding hypnotic subjects.
In
summary, the enactment of hidden or dissociated selves. by hypnotic and
nonhypnotic subjects involves strategic, rule-governed self-presentation. In these studies, information about the
characteristics of the hidden selves was provided by experimental instructions,
and subjects guided their experiences and behaviors in terms of these role
prescriptions. Thus, the characteristics
of hidden self enactments varied as a function of the performance expectations
transmitted to subjects.
Past-Life Regression Experiments
Several
studies have examined factors that influence the formation of multiple selves
by using the phenomenon of past-life hypnotic regression. Although some believers in reincarnation hold
that people can be hypnotically regressed back to previous lives (e.g.,
Wambaugh, 1979), the available evidence suggests instead that past-life
experiences are fantasy constructions (Baker, 1992; Spanos, Menary, Gabora,
DuBreuil, & Dewhirst, 199 1). These
experiences are important because they shed light on the processes by which
people come to treat their fantasies as real and because past-life identities
are similar in many respects to the multiple identities of MPD patients. Like MPD patients, subjects who exhibit
past-life identities behave as if they are inhabited by more than one
self. Like the secondary selves of MPD
patients, those exhibited by past-life responders often display moods and
personality characteristics that are different from the person's primary self,
have a different name than the primary self, and report memories of which the
primary self was previously unaware.
Just as MPD patients come to believe that their alter identities are
real personalities rather than self-generated fantasies, many of the subjects
who enact past lives continue to believe in the reality of their past lives
after termination of the hypnotic procedures.
Kampman
(1976) found that 41% of highly hypnotisable subjects manifested evidence of a
new identity and called themselves by different names when hypnotically
regressed beyond their birth. Contrary
to the notion that the development of multiple identities is a sign of mental
illness, Kampman's past life responders scored higher on measures of
psychological health than did subjects who failed to exhibit a past life.
In
a series of experiments, Spanos, Menary, et al. (1991) also obtained past-life
identity enactments after hypnotic regression suggestions. Frequently, the past-life identities were
elaborate, had their own names, and described their lives in great detail. Subjects who reported past lives scored
higher on indexes of hypnotizability and fantasy proneness, but no higher on
indexes of psychopathology, than those who did not exhibit a past life.
The
characteristics that subjects attributed to their past lives were influenced by
experimenter-transmitted expectations.
For instance, subjects provided with prehypnotic information about the
likely characteristics of their identities were much more likely than controls
to incorporate these characteristics into their descriptions of their past-life
selves (Spanos, Menary, et al., 1991, Experiment 2). In a different study (Spanos, Menary, et al.,
1991, Experiment 3), some subjects were informed prehypnotically that children
in past eras were frequently abused, whereas those in the other condition were
given no information about abuse. The
past-life identities of subjects given abuse information reported significantly
higher levels of childhood abuse than did the past-life identities of control
subjects. In summary, these studies
indicate that the personal attributes and memory reports elicited from subjects
who enact past-life identities are influenced by the beliefs and expectations
conveyed by the experimenter/hypnotist.
Subjects shape the attributes and biographies attributed to their past-life
identities to correspond to their understandings of what significant others
believe these characteristics to be.
The
extent to which subjects assigned credibility to their past life identities
correlated significantly with the degree to which they placed credence in
reincarnation before the experiment and the extent to which they expected to
experience a real past life. However,
subjects' beliefs in the reality of their past lives were also influenced by information
from the experimenter. Spanos, Menary,
et al. (1991, Experiment 4) informed subjects in one condition that past-life
identities were interesting fantasies but were certainly not evidence of real
past lives. Those in another condition
were informed that reincarnation was a scientifically credible notion. Subjects in the two conditions were equally
likely to enact past-life identities, but those assigned to the imaginary
creation condition assigned significantly less credibility to these identities
than did those told that reincarnation was scientifically credible. In short, prior information from authority
figures influences not only the characteristics and memories that people
attribute to their multiple identities but also the degree to which they come
to believe in the reality of these identities.
Taken
together, the experimental data indicate that multiple identities are social
creations that can be elicited easily from many normal people. When the identity to be constructed is
relatively complex, as in past-life regression studies, subjects draw on
information from a wide array of sources (e.g., TV shows, historical novels,
aspects of their own past, and wish-fulfilling daydreams) to flesh out the
newly constructed identity and to provide it with the history and
characteristics that are called for by their understanding of the current task
demands. These studies indicate that the
development of multiple identities is not related to psychopathology and that
men are as adept as women at creating such identities. Although none of these studies obtained
information about whether subjects had been abused as children, the fact that
psychopathology failed to predict either the development of these identities or
the extent to which subjects construed them to be real rather than imagined
makes it unlikely that early abuse played an important role in these regards.
Cross-Cultural Studies on Spirit Possession
Multiple
self enactments occur in most but not all cultures (Bourguignon, 1976). In many traditional societies and in some subcultural
contexts in North American society, multiple self enactments take the form of
spirit possession. In these cases, it is
believed that the human occupant of the body is temporarily displaced by
another self or selves that are defined as spirits who temporarily take over
control of the body. Frequently, the
human self claims amnesia for the periods during which the spirit selves are in
control (Bourguignon, 1976; Lewis, 1987).
The
frequency with which possession occurs varies greatly from one society to
another. In some societies that hold
possession beliefs, only a relatively small percentage of the population is
ever defined as actually possessed. For
instance, Wijesinghe, Dissanayake, and Mendis (1 976) reported an incidence of
0.5% for a semiurban population in Sri Lanka; Carstairs and Kapur (1976) found
a period prevalence rate in a rural population on the west coast of South India
of 2.8%; and Venkataramaiah, Mallikadunaiah, Chandra, Rao, and Reddy (1981)
reported a prevalence of 3.7% in a different South Indian rural
population. In other societies, the
rates of possession are extremely high.
For instance, in the villages of the Malagasy, speakers of Mayotte, Lambek
(1980) reported that 39% of the adult women and 8% of the adult men were considered
to be possessed. Relatedly, Harper
(1963) reported that 20% of the women among the Havik Brahmins in Mysore,
India, experienced possession, and Boddy (1988) found that in different years
42% and 47% of ever-married women more than 15 years of age in the village of
Hofriyat in Northern Sudan had succumbed to possession. When considering only women between the ages
of 35 and 55 years, Boddy (1988) reported that 66.6% bad experienced
possession.
In
most traditional societies that hold possession beliefs, possession occurs much
more frequently in females than in males.
This, however, is not invariably the case, and in some societies
possession occurs with equal or almost equal frequency in the two sexes (Lewis,
1987). If nothing else, the marked differences
between societies in rates of possession, coupled with the very high rates of
possession in some societies, should make one wary of explanations of multiple
identity development that emphasize the importance of stable personality or
cognitive characteristics (such as high fantasy proneness or high
hypnotizability) as necessary predisposing factors. Sex ratios and the proportion of community
members affected also vary widely in some North American groups that display
spirit possession.
Two North American Examples of Spirit
Possession
One
relatively common form of possession experience in North American society is
religious glossolalia (Hine, 1969).
Glossolalia involves semantically meaningless vocal utterances that
sound language-like and that are sometimes mistaken by naive listeners as a
foreign language. Glossolalia is
frequently spoken in certain Christian religious settings. It may be accompanied by dramatic behaviors
including convulsions, profuse sweating, eye closure, and an apparent loss of
consciousness, but it often occurs in the absence of all such dramatic
accompaniments (Spanos & Hewitt, 1979).
Whether or not glossolalia is accompanied by dramatic displays appears
to depend on the expectations of the glossolalics and their audience and the
norms of the particular setting in which it is displayed. Traditionally, glossolalia has been
interpreted in Christian circles as possession by the Holy Spirit, who speaks
his own language (the glossolalia) through the possessed person.
Glossolalia
is learned behavior, and rates of glossolalia differ dramatically across
different religious groups as a function of expectations concerning who will
and will not manifest the phenomenon. In
some congregations, glossolalia is encouraged and occurs in all or almost all
members. In other congregations, it is
relatively rare (Samarin, 1972).
Glossolalia can be easily learned through modelling and practice
(Spanos, Cross, Lepage, & Coristine, 1986), and congregations that
encourage glossolalia typically provide the novice with much encouragement and
coaching and multiple opportunities to closely observe other glossolalics
(Maloney & Lovekin, 1985).
Glossolalics do not score higher than nonglossolalics on measures of
hypnotizability, imaginative activity, or psychopathology (Richardson, 197 3;
Spanos & Hewitt, 1979), and the ability to learn glossolalia is unrelated
to either hypnotizability or imaginative activity (Spanos, Cross, et al.,
1986). Glossolalia can occur with equal
frequency in men and women, and when sex differences do occur they reflect
local custom rather than intrinsic gender differences (Samarin, 1972).
In
some religious communities, the first manifestation of glossolalia is
interpreted as a sign of salvation that signals full acceptance into the
religious group. In describing their
conversion, new members typically draw a sharp distinction between their new
(post-conversion) identity and their old life of sin.
These
members frequently reconstruct their biographies to accentuate differences
between their pre-conversion and post-conversion identities, In so doing, they
emphasize their first glossolalic experience as a marker of what they and their
community view as a critical transition point between a discarded sinful
identity and a new consecrated identity (Hine, 1970; Maloney & Lovekin,
1985).
Another
form of possession with a long history in Western societies is spirit
mediumship (R. L. Moore, 1977). During the late 19th century, at the height
of interest in mediumship in America, spirit mediums were often female, and
adoption of the medium role was a vehicle through which women could circumvent
some of the restrictions associated with the female role and earn an
independent living. North American
mediums usually became possessed by a control spirit who served as an
intermediary to the spirit world and who summoned the spirits of departed
relatives, who in turn possessed and spoke through the medium. Typically mediums reported amnesia for the
period during which the spirits occupied their bodies.
Although
less common than previously, spirit mediums continue to operate in many North
American cities. Biscop (1981, 1985)
conducted a participant observation study of a spiritist church in Canada and
documented the process by which novices were socialised into the role of
medium. Part of this process involved
formal teaching, but much of it involved repeated opportunities to observe more
experienced mediums, coupled with encouragement in small-group settings in
which novices were provided with helpful feedback. Mediumship involves learning how to
"read" the client to obtain information about the deceased that can
then be fed back to the client as proof of the deceased's survival. At times, mediums go to great lengths and use
much trickery to obtain information and provide convincing performances (Keene,
1976). Biscop (1981) interviewed six
North American mediums at length. Most
reported relatively happy childhoods, and, in marked contrast to MPD patients,
only one reported that she had been sexually abused as a child.
The
characteristics of possession displays in other cultures also vary greatly both
between societies and within societies.
Possession is not a unitary phenomenon; it differs dramatically
depending on the status of the possessed person, the context in which the
possession occurs, and the meaning attributed to the possession both by the
possessed individual and by his or her audience (Krippner, in press; Lewis,
1987).
Ritual Possession
In
many societies, spirit possession occurs as part of helping rituals. The medium becomes possessed by a spirit or
by successive spirits, and it is the spirits who diagnose the client, prescribe
treatments, or offer advice for problems in living. The rituals can be private consultations
involving only the medium and client or public ceremonies involving one or more
mediums and large audiences (Lee, 1989; Lewis, 1987). The structure of these rituals illustrates
the social nature of multiple identity enactments and the dependence of such
enactments on social validation. The
medium and the audience hold complementary expectations concerning the
behaviors that define the medium as possessed by a particular spirit and the
behaviors that define members of the audience as validating the spirit
presentations. The medium presents as a
specific spirit by enacting the behavioural displays that the audience
identifies with that spirit. The
audience, in turn, validates the medium's presentation by responding in a
manner that is congruent with the particular spirit identity being presented
(Firth, 1967; Lambek, 1988; Lee, 1989).
For
instance, the transition from the medium's human personality to that of a
spirit is marked by readily identifiable signs (e.g., shaking and eye closure)
that the audience has learned to interpret in terms of spirit possession. Similarly, the transition from one spirit
identity to another is marked by recognisable changes in behavior such as
changes in voice, personality, and dress.
In some cultures, particular spirits dance to some tunes but not others;
in others, each change in spirit identity is marked by replacing a scarf of one
colour with a scarf of another. When
behaving as a warrior spirit, the medium struts to and fro in an aggressive and
threatening manner and the audience responds accordingly by being quiet and
respectful. When presenting as a tiger
spirit, the medium may walk on all fours and growl while the audience backs
away. When presenting as a risqué
spirit, the medium jokes with the audience, which responds with relaxation and
laughter (Krippner, 1989; Lambek, 1989; Lee, 1989; Saunders, 1977). The result of these mutually supporting
interactions is the construction of spirit possession as a social reality
(Schieffelin, 1985).
Often,
ritual possession ceremonies involve a number of people who are possessed
simultaneously (Lewis, 1987).
Furthermore, in some ceremonies, a single spirit can move from
possessing one person to possessing another.
However, the same spirit cannot possess two people simultaneously. Thus, ceremonies that involve the
simultaneous possession of several people by spirits that move between mediums
involve a good deal of co-ordination.
The various mediums must be aware of their own performances as well as
those of other mediums to keep their changing roles distinct (Lambek, 1988;
Lee, 1989).
The
rule-governed nature of ritual possession is also illustrated by the
preparation required for a convincing performance. The props for differentiating spirit
identities must be readily available.
For instance, when presenting as a tiger spirit, one medium would bite
and suck at the patient's body until he produced from his mouth a black
substance that he called black pus and blood.
The blood he probably produced by biting his own cheek, and the
"black pus" he produced by putting ashes into his mouth before the
ceremony and before becoming possessed (Peters, 1981).
Becoming
a spirit medium usually involves an extensive socialisation process, and once
an individual becomes a medium periodic possession may be a lifelong
occurrence. In some societies,
mediumship runs in families, and particular spirits move from possessing a
parent to possessing one of his or her children. In other cases, mediums are former patients
who apprentice with their healer.
Frequently, there are a number of different paths into mediumship within
the same society. Regardless of the path
taken, however, the medium must learn the rules required to give convincing
performances that meet the expectations of clients and other audience members
(Krippner, 1989; Morton, 1977; Peters, 1981).
Becoming
a spirit medium, like becoming a glossolalic, sometimes involves the possessed
organising their biographies to correspond with implicit societal conceptions
concerning the meaning of possession. In
some societies, for instance, the possession careers of mediums are described
in highly stereotyped fashion and include a series of stock background events
(e.g., fleeing into the wilderness) that explain why they were singled out by
the spirits for possession (Morton, 1977).
Investigators of mediums in traditional cultures (Krippner, 1989;
Leacock & Leacock, 1972) have often commented that these individuals
usually appear to be well adjusted, mentally healthy, and competent.
The
spirit possession enactments of mediums are responsive to sociocultural
changes, and in some circumstances possession becomes a vehicle for expressing
resistance to externally imposed authority (Stoller, 1989). For instance, during the French colonial
period, native African mediums among the Songhay became possessed by a new
group of spirits that aped colonial officials and burlesqued French colonial
society with displays of exaggerated and satiric behavior. Later, when French rule was replaced by a
puritanical Islamic state, mediums expressed their resistance by enacting
scatological and overtly sexual possession displays that violated the official
Islamic moral code (Stoller, 1989). The
responsiveness of possession displays to cultural changes illustrates the
constructive and goal-directed nature of possession and the importance of
ongoing situational factors (as opposed to idiosyncratic psychological ones) in
determining both the character of possession displays and the historical changes
that occur in the nature of those displays.
Peripheral Possession
Lewis
(1987) distinguished between central and peripheral possession. In central possession, the medium is
possessed by the major deities of the society, and the possession performances
serve to publicly reaffirm and support the central values of the society. Typically the medium in central possession is
a respected member of the community. In
contrast, peripheral possession afflicts socially marginal and oppressed
members of the community. In this case,
the possessing spirits are capricious and often amoral members of the pantheon,
and possession is associated with illness, emotional distress, or both.
Lewis
(1987) hypothesised that peripheral possession often constitutes a strategy
used by the socially powerless to manipulate their social superiors. Typically, peripheral possession occurs in
people low in the social hierarchy who are experiencing high levels of
psychological or interpersonal stress.
The stress is often manifested in psychophysiological symptoms (e.g.,
headaches) that are interpreted as initial manifestations of possession. The possessing spirit makes numerous demands
that must be met by the family of the possessed. Possession is considered involuntary, and the
disruptive behavior and unusual demands are attributed to the possessing spirit
rather than the possessed person.
Frequently, peripheral possession is chronic, and new or recurring
stresses lead to a recurrence of symptoms (Saunders, 1977; Ward, 1989).
Many
traditional societies are strongly patriarchal, and the women in these
societies often have few rights and are hemmed in by many social
restrictions. Consequently, in most of
these societies it is women rather then men who resort to peripheral possession
as an interpersonal strategy for improving their lot. For example, a woman who is in an unhappy
marriage to an inattentive or brutal husband and lives in a culture that
restricts married women almost exclusively to the home may become possessed by
a spirit who demands an expensive public ceremony that includes rich
delicacies, new clothing, and interaction with other women. Moreover, when enacting the spirit role, the
woman can voice unflattering and insulting remarks to her husband that would
not be tolerated if they were defined as coming from her rather than from her
spirit (Constantinides, 1977).
Possession is a public event, and the norms of the community may demand
that the husband abide by the requests of the spirit despite his personal
feelings toward his wife and despite the substantial expense involved.
In
many societies where peripheral possession occurs, the aim of treatment is not
to expel the spirit but to bring it under control. Frequently, this taming process is
accomplished by the possessed joining a possession cult (Lewis, 1987). Here the possessed individual joins with
other possessed individuals under the tutelage of a shaman. The shaman is herself possessed but has
learned to control her possessing spirits and use them as spiritual
advisors. The women in the cult meet
regularly to hold feasts and dances in honour of the spirits and to seek
spiritual advice from the spirits of the shaman (Lewis, 1987; Morton, 1977). Frequently, these cult groups foster a high
level of cohesiveness and appear to provide tangible psychological benefits for
their adherents (Galanter, 1990; Morton, 1977).
Rates
of peripheral possession can change within a culture. In societies that condone possession beliefs,
cultural changes that increase stress levels often produce increased rates of
possession (Ackerman & Lee, 1981; Phoon, 1982; Sharp, 1990; Teoh, Soewondo,
& Sidharta, 1975). For instance,
several investigators (Ackerman & Lee, 1981; Ong, 1988; Phoon, 1982) have
described small epidemics of possession in female Malaysian factory
workers. These women frequently carry
out boring, repetitive work for very low wages under poor working
conditions. The anti-union policies of
the factories effectively prevent organised protest. Under these circumstances, epidemics of
spirit possession that involve convulsions and bizarre behavior become a way of
venting distress and frustration, obtaining time off from work, and rebelling
against authority. Because the spirits
rather than the possessed women are blamed for the disturbances, possession
displays are a safe, albeit indirect, way of expressing grievances. In a factory studied by Ackerman and Lee
(1981), increases in the frequency of possession episodes followed a change
from relaxed to stricter management.
Moreover, the possession displays occurred only among the Malaysian
workers whose cultural beliefs made possession an acceptable vehicle for the
expression of dissatisfaction. Chinese
and Indian workers in the same factory never exhibited displays of possession.
Epidemic
possession affects numerous people in close proximity within a short period of
time and, therefore, cannot be accounted for by theories of multiplicity that
emphasize idiosyncratic psychological causes.
The contagion that occurs in these cases results from social factors,
from the common understandings held by participants about what constitute
legitimate means of expressing dissatisfaction, and from the effects of
observing displays of possession and the consequences of those displays.
In
summary, possession phenomena underscore the rule-governed and social nature of
multiple identity displays. Enactments
of spirit possession are learned patterns of social responding. Possessed individuals enact spirit identities
that correspond to their understandings and expectations of possession. Possession enactments are public and involve
interaction with an audience that legitimates the enactments. These enactments occur in a wide range of
circumstances, are carried out by very different kinds of people, and serve a
number of different social functions.
Possession is sometimes symptomatic of severe stress and accompanied by
symptoms of psychopathology. At other
times, however, it is enacted by well-adjusted individuals who do not manifest
high levels of psychopathology.
Historical Manifestations of Demonic Possession
The
idea that demons can enter into people and take over their functioning entered
Western European history as an accompaniment of Christianity (Spanos,
1983a). Although information concerning
the rate of possession in earlier centuries is sparse, it seems clear that this
rate varied dramatically in different historical eras (Oesterreich, 1966). Possession and exorcism were frequently used
as proselytising tools to impress and convert unbelievers. Consequently, possession appears to have been
a relatively common occurrence in the early church while Christianity struggled
for supremacy among numerous competing religions. After Christianity became th,- state religion
of the Roman Empire, the frequency of possession and exorcisms appears to have
waned.
Beginning
in the 11th. century with the gradual breakdown of feudalism, Western Europe
experienced increased politico-religious turmoil that, in the 16th century,
culminated in the Reformation and in the break-up of Western Christianity into
competing sects (Russell, 1980). This
period also saw the development and elaboration of the mythology of Satanism. According to the tenets of this mythology,
there existed an international satanic conspiracy bent on destroying
Christianity. The agents of this
conspiracy were witches who supposedly worshipped Satan at secret meetings
where they desecrated the symbols of Christian worship and engaged in
cannibalism, murder, and sexual orgies (Cohn, 1975). Modern historians have rejected the notion
that there actually was a satanic conspiracy or that those accused of
witchcraft belonged to a large-scale conspiracy. Instead, the idea of a satanic conspiracy
existed only in the imagination. It
existed first in the imagination of cultural elites who established the
administrative machinery and legal categories that made satanic witchcraft a
crime and then spread down the social scale to become part of the
taken-for-granted belief system of much of the populous (Cohn, 1975; Larner,
198 1; Russell, 1980; Spanos, 1978).
Possession
and exorcism again became common between the 15th and 17th centuries because,
during this period, possession became associated with witchcraft. Both Catholics and Protestants believed that
witches, through Satan's intercession, could send demons to possess
people. However, the indwelling demons
could be coerced by authorities to name the witch that sent them. The accused witch could then be arrested,
tortured into confessing her involvement in a non-existent satanic conspiracy,
and, in many cases, executed. Thus,
during this period demoniacs frequently functioned as witch finders, and those
who controlled the demoniacs had a powerful weapon to use against political,
social, or personal rivals (Spanos, 1978).
The Socialization of Demoniacs
The
idea of demon possession was taken for granted in early modern Europe, and the
major components of that role were well-known (Thomas, 1971). The subtle aspects of the role were
transmitted through the demoniac's exposure to clerical experts. Demonic possession was used as one
explanation for certain physical symptoms or for behavior that was socially
disruptive or considered abnormal.
During the initial stages of possession, the demoniac's symptoms were
often ambiguous. Frequently, these
symptoms began to correspond to "official" stereotypes of demonic
possession as the demoniac gained information about those stereotypes (Spanos,
1983b; Spanos & Gottlieb, 1976; Walker, 1981).
The
symptom displays of the possessed sometimes varied with local beliefs and
practices, and these variations demonstrate the social and rule-governed nature
of possession. For instance, both
Catholic and Protestant demoniacs regularly convulsed and displayed a variety
of sensory-motor deficits. In addition,
Catholic demoniacs invariably exhibited direct evidence of indwelling demon
selves. These demon selves spoke in
voices different from that of the possessed person, had their own names, and
displayed their own unique demonic personalities. Protestant demoniacs of the same period
rarely displayed demon selves (Walker, 1981).
Protestant-Catholic
variation in the frequency of demon self enactments reflected the different
practices toward the possessed adopted by these religions. During Catholic exorcisms, the priest
communicated directly with the indwelling demon. The priest made a clear distinction between
the possessing demons and the person possessed.
When questioning a demon, the exorcist expected to be answered by the
demon and not by the person possessed (Oesterreich, 1966). Before beginning the exorcism rite, the
priest contacted and questioned the demons to obtain their names, number, reasons
for possessing the person, hour they entered the body, and length of time they
proposed to stay (Kelly, 1974; "The Roman Ritual of Exorcism;' 1614/
1976). During the exorcism, the demons
were often questioned repeatedly and at great length about their motives,
earthly accomplices, status in the social structure of hell, and so forth
(e.g., Michaelis, 1613). In short,
Catholic exorcism procedures strongly cued demon self enactments as a central
component of the demonic role. In
contrast, Protestants rarely used formal exorcism procedures because direct
communication with demons was shunned as sinful (Thomas, 1971). In place of exorcism, Protestant demoniacs
were treated with prayer and fasting, procedures that were much less likely to
elicit demon self enactments.
Detailed
information concerning role prescriptions was conveyed to both Catholic and
Protestant demoniacs outside of the exorcism situation. The sources of this information could include
explicit coaching by parties who held a vested interest in the demoniacs giving
convincing performances, exposure to other more practised demoniacs, and
conversations about the occurrence and timing of symptoms that were held in the
demoniac's presence (Harsnett, 1599, 1603; Thomas, 1971; Walker, 1981).
A
number of potent social psychological factors converged in leading potential
demoniacs to define themselves as possessed., These individuals shared the same
belief system as the community that labelled them and, therefore, tended to
interpret their own illness of behavioural deviations in the same terms as
their neighbours and clerical superiors (Thomas, 1971). In some cases, demoniacs were made dependent
for the satisfaction of their physical and social needs on those who labelled
them. The labellers consistently
interpreted the experiences of demoniacs in terms of possession and isolated
the demoniacs from others who might offer nondemonic interpretations of these
events (Spanos, 1983a).
Individuals
sometimes denied that they were possessed; however, these denials were
routinely construed by authorities as indications of a wily demon attempting to
escape divine punishment. Continued
refusal to define oneself as possessed and act accordingly frequently led to
threats of perpetual damnation and sometimes to punishment administered in the
guise of benevolently motivated attempts to free hapless victims from demonic
control (Harsnett, 1599; Spanos, 1983a).
Reasons for Adopting the Possessed Role
People
did not always avoid being labelled demonically possessed. Like sufferers of peripheral possession in
other cultures, the demonically possessed could use their enactments
strategically. Those who became
demoniacs usually had little social power, were hemmed in by social
restrictions, and had few sanctioned avenues for protesting or improving on
their situations. Given the patriarchal
and misogynistic culture of early modem Europe, it is not surprising that adult
demoniacs were much more frequently women than men (Oesterreich, 1966). Child demoniacs, on the other hand, were
frequently of both sexes (Spanos, 1983a).
For
the socially powerless, demonic possession offered numerous advantages. Its adoption could lead to a dramatic rise in
social status. On one hand, demoniacs
were viewed as the helpless victims of satanic influence and consequently
received sympathetic attention and a reduced workload. On the other hand, they were sometimes
treated as awesome seers whose affliction placed them in direct contact with
the supernatural and whose performances commanded fearful respect and attention. Demoniacs sometimes became the star
attractions in what the community considered a deadly serious combat between
the forces of heaven and hell (Spanos, 1983a; Walker, 1981). Like peripheral possession in other cultures
and MPD in North American culture, demonic possession was often chronic. Frequently, these individuals were possessed
by many demons that had to be individually exorcised over a long period of
time. Moreover, even the successful
exorcism of all the demons was no guarantee that they would not return. Thus, once possession had been legitimated,
it remained an option that could be used as the situation required.
Like
the ritual ceremonies conducted by spirit mediums in other cultures, the
performances of demoniacs often required forethought and the judicious use of
props. For instance, the spitting up of
pins or nails that were supposedly used by indwelling demons to torture the
demoniac internally was a fairly regular feature of English and Continental
possession cases (Notestein, 1911; Oesterreich, 1966). Obviously, the demoniacs had to place the
pins in their mouths before their performance in anticipation of spitting them
out later and in anticipation of the effect that such displays would produce in
their audience.
In
summary, demonic enactments constituted co-ordinated, goal-directed
self-presentations aimed at conveying and sustaining the impression that the
actors were possessed by evil spirits.
As in cross-cultural manifestations of spirit possession, a major
feature of the demonic role involved conveying the impression that behaviors
were no longer governed by the actor.
However, conveying this impression convincingly required that demoniacs
retain behavioural control and gear their enactments to contextual demands in a
manner consistent with their audience's conception of what constituted
possession.
Multiple Personality Disorder
Cases
that today would be labelled as MPD were reported infrequently during the first
three quarters of the 19th century. An
increase in the number of cases was reported in the last quarter of the 19th
and the early years of the 20th century, and most of these were from either
France or the United States (Hacking, 1986).
By the end of the first 2 decades of the 20th century, the number of
cases had dropped substantially, and from the 1920s to 1970 only a handful were
reported world-wide. Since 1970, the
number of cases reported has increased astronomically, and thousands have now
been reported (Putnam, 1989).
Investigators who are sympathetic to the MPD diagnosis are particularly
likely to find high rates of occurrence of the disorder. For instance, Bliss, Larson, and Nakashima
(1983) reported that 60% of the 45 patients admitted to a single inpatient ward
with auditory hallucinations were actually suffering from MPD. Bliss and Jeppsen (1985) reported that
approximately 8 of 50 (16%) sequentially admitted inpatients on an acute
psychiatric ward and 9 of 100 (9%) private psychiatric outpatients suffered
from MPD. Ross, Anderson, Fleisher, and
Norton (1991) diagnosed 3.3% of a sample of 299 psychiatric inpatients as
suffering from MPD. Despite the high
rates of occurrence found in these studies and the high rates reported more
informally by numerous other clinicians who are strong supporters of the MPD
diagnosis (e.g., R. B. Allison & Schwarz, 1980; Braun, 1984; Kluft, 1982),
some studies have reported very low rates of occurrence. Thus, Merskey and Buhrich (1975) found no MPD
among 89 patients diagnosed with dissociative or conversion disorders. In addition, a number of highly experienced
clinicians with many years of practice report never having seen a case of MPD
(Chordoff, 1987; Merskey, 1992).
At
present, MPD appears to be a culture-bound syndrome. The explosion of cases since 1970 has been
largely restricted to North America.
Despite its turn-of-the-century prominence as a center for the study of
MPD, the diagnosis is very rarely made in modern France. It is also very rare in Great Britain
(Aldridge-Morris, 1989; Fahy, 1988), Russia (R. M. Allison, 1991), and India
(Adityhanjee & Khandelwal, 1989), and a recent survey in Japan failed to
uncover even a single case (Takahashi, 1990).
Modestin
(1992) surveyed all of the psychiatrists in Switzerland concerning the
frequency with which they had seen patients with MPD. Depending on how it was calculated, the
prevalence rate ranged between 0.5% and 1.0%. More Interesting, Modestin noted
that 90% of the respondents had never seen a case of MPD, whereas 3
psychiatrists had each seen more than 20 MPD patients. From the data presented in this article, it
appears that 66% of the MPD diagnoses were made by only 6 of 655 (0.09%)
psychiatrists. Unlike North American
samples in which women are at least three times more likely than men to receive
an MPD diagnosis, Modestin found that 51% of the MPD diagnoses were given to
men.
The
historical changes in the prevalence of MPD, the substantial national
differences in prevalence and in gender ratios, and the large differences in
the frequency with which different clinicians make the diagnosis are difficult
to account for parsimoniously in terms of a disease perspective. They are, however, reminiscent of the
historical changes in the prevalence of demonic possession, the large
cross-cultural differences in the prevalence of spirit possession, and the
large differences in rates of glossolalia seen between different churches in
North America. In short, the prevalence
data for MPD suggest that MPD, like possession phenomena, is a social creation
that varies in frequency as a function of the expectations for its occurrence
that are transmitted to patients. The
most important sources of such expectations are therapists committed to the MPD
diagnosis, but other sources (e.g., media) have become increasingly important
as information about MPD has infused into the wider culture.
An
alternative to the social creation hypothesis holds that the incidence of MPD
has not really increased. According to
this view, MPD was previously (and often continues to be) misdiagnosed as
schizophrenia, psychopathy, and various other disorders (R. B. Allison &
Schwarz, 1980; Rosenbaum, 1980; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, 1989). Purportedly, the current increased prevalence
of the disorder is apparent rather than real and reflects the fact that actual
MPD cases are incorrectly diagnosed less frequently than previously. This account is also sometimes used to
explain national differences in MPD rates (Altrocchi, 1992). According to this view, psychiatrists in
other countries have not yet developed the diagnostic acumen required to
recognise MPD.
A
major problem with this account is that the symptoms of MPD are, in fact, quite
distinctive. A patient who calls herself
Mary on one day and Jane on another and who behaves very differently as Mary
than as Jane is unlikely to go unrecognised as a candidate for an MPD diagnosis
by even an inexperienced clinician. To
deal with this difficulty, proponents of the disease model argue that such patients
are skilled at hiding their multiplicity and reveal it only when those with
acumen enough to correctly interpret the subtle signs use diagnostic procedures
that bring forth the multiple identities.
In fact, the proponents of the MPD diagnosis agree that most patients
who eventually receive this diagnosis do not enter therapy complaining about
multiple personalities, do not exhibit clear-cut evidence of multiplicity, and
do not know that they possess alter identities (R. B. Allison, 1978; Franklin,
1988; Wilbur, 1984). These investigators
regard MPD as a "hidden syndrome" in which 80% of such patients were
unaware of the existence of alters before entering treatment with the therapist
who uncovered their multiplicity (Kluft, 1985).
From a socio-cognitive perspective, these data suggest instead that the
procedures used to diagnose MPD often create rather than discover multiplicity.
Teaching Multiplicity
Proponents
of the MPD diagnosis have described a large and diverse number of signs that
supposedly might indicate this disorder and that can be used to justify probing
for confirmation. Some of these signs
include depression, somatoform symptoms, headaches, periods of missing time,
impaired concentration, hallucinations, sexual dysfunctions, fatigue, and drug
abuse (Coons et al., 1988; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, 1989). There are many more. R. B. Allison (1978) even suggested that a
smooth complexion might indicate MPD because the regular switching of alter personalities
supposedly prevents the formation of wrinkles.
Even this truncated list makes it clear that a large set of presenting
symptoms can be used as possible indicators of MPD.
Once
the diagnosis is suspected, it is common practice to ask leading and explicit
questions in an attempt to confirm it.
Putnam (1989), for example, asked such questions as "Do you ever
feel as if you are not alone, as if there is someone else or some other part
watching you?" (p. 90). Others
apply stronger pressure. Merskey (1992)
recently reviewed a large number of MPD cases from the 20th century and earlier
that provided information about treatment procedures and symptomatology. His review included well-known cases such as
those of Sally Beauchamp (Prince, 1908), The
Three Faces of Eve (Thigpen & Cleckley, 1957), and Sybil (Schreiber, 1973), as well as many lesser known ones. Merskey (1992) found that highly leading and
suggestive procedures have long been routine in the diagnosis of MPD. In some cases, therapists insisted to
doubting patients that they were multiples and even supplied the patients with
names for their alters. Along these
lines, it is worth noting that Allison and Schwarz (1980) contended that
patients are frequently reluctant to accept that they are multiples and, under
these circumstances, should be actively persuaded by their therapist.
Perhaps
the most common procedure used to elicit evidence of multiplicity is the use of
highly leading hypnotic interviews during which alter personalities are
explicitly suggested and explicitly asked to "come forth" and talk
with the therapist (Allison & Schwarz, 1980; Bliss, 1980, 1986; Brandsma
& Ludwig, 1974). In fact, R. B.
Allison (1978) stated that the major difference between therapists who diagnose
MPD and those who do not is the use by the former of hypnotic procedures for
uncovering hidden memories. Wilbur
(1984) described the process: "The patient is hypnotised and each
alternate, in turn, is asked to tell what precipitated it into the life of the
birth personality" (p. 28). Bliss
(1986) elaborated on the procedure:
“I then suggest that the [hypnotised]
patient look into her mind to see if there is anyone or anything there. If anything or a person is identified I want
to know who it is. [Once an alter has been identified] I then make a rapid survey
by asking the personality . . . if she has a name; how long she has been there;
the patient's age when she [the alter] came; whether the patient knows her;
whether she ever takes over the body; whether she ever directs or influences
the patient when the patient has the body; her mission or function; and whether
there are other people back there.” (pp. 196-197)
Given
the highly leading nature of these procedures, it comes as no surprise that
there often are "other people back there," and they are identified
and surveyed in the same manner. The
flavour of such interviews is conveyed in the following verbatim excerpt from a
hypnotic interview with a suspected murderer named Ken Bianchi. Because it was conducted in a forensic
context, Bianchi's interview was videotaped.
After a hypnotic induction procedure, the clinician proceeded as
follows:
“I've talked a bit to Ken but I think that
perhaps there might be another part of Ken that I haven't talked to. And I would like to communicate with that
part. And I would like that other part
to come and talk to me. . . . And when you are here, lift the left hand off the
chair to signal to me that you are here.
Would you please come part so I can talk to you. . . . Part would you
come and lift Ken's hand to indicate to me that you are here? . . . Would you
talk to me part by saying "I'm here."“ (Schwarz, 1981, pp. 142-143)
During
this interview, Bianchi displayed a second personality named Steve who
confessed to the murders with which Ken had been charged. The clinician who conducted this interview
pointed out that he did nothing unusual in this case and used such interview
procedures regularly to diagnose MPD (Watkins, 1984). In other words, leading hypnotic interviews
that repeatedly inform the patient that he or she has other parts that can be
addressed and communicated with as if they were separate people are used
routinely when diagnosing MPD.
Spanos,
Weekes, and Bertrand (1985) used the hypnotic interview that was employed with
Bianchi to test the hypothesis that such procedures can provide even naive
subjects with the information required to enact multiple identities. The subjects were college students asked to
pretend that they were in Bianchi's situation, had been accused of committing a
series of murders, and had been remanded for a psychiatric interview. Subjects were told nothing about multiple
personality. They were simply instructed
to use whatever background information they possessed and whatever they could
glean from their interview to behave the way they believed an accused in that
situation would behave.
Subjects
in one group were administered an interview modelled closely on the one used
with Bianchi. Role-playing control
subjects were also interviewed. These
subjects were told that personality was complex and involved walled-off
thoughts and feelings, but the interviewing "psychiatrist" never
informed the controls that they possessed another part that could be
communicated with directly.
Most
of the role players given the Bianchi interview enacted symptoms of MPD by (a)
adopting a different name, (b) referring to their primary personality in the
third person, and (c) displaying amnesia for their alter personalities after
termination of the hypnotic interview.
None of the role-playing controls displayed any of these symptoms.
In
a second session, the "psychiatrist" again contacted the alter
personalities of the role-playing multiples.
These subjects maintained their role successfully in the second session
by exhibiting marked and consistent differences between their primary and alter
personalities on a variety of psychological tests. Role-playing controls performed similarly on
the two administrations of the test. A
replication of this study by Rabinowitz (1989) yielded similar findings.
Spanos,
Weekes, Menary, and Bertrand (1986) extended the findings of Spanos, Weekes,
and Bertrand (1985) and Rabinowitz (1989) by exposing role-playing multiples to
interviews that focused on their childhood experiences. The task of the role players was not, of
course, to describe their actual childhoods but, to use cues gleaned from the
interviews to present themselves as actual patients. Like the histories given by actual MPD
patients, the role-playing multiples gave negatively toned descriptions of
childhood, described their parents as punitive and rejecting, described an
early onset (before 10 years of age) for their alter personalities, and
described their alters as "taking over" to handle difficult
situations and express strong emotions.
The findings of these studies are straightforward. They demonstrate that the interviewing
procedures used routinely to diagnose MPD convey all of the information
required to allow even psychiatrically unsophisticated subjects to enact the
cardinal symptoms of multiplicity.
All
of the subjects in these studies had been explicitly asked to fake their
responses, and, consequently, it can be argued that people who were not faking
would be unlikely to develop multiple identities after exposure to leading
hypnotic interviews. This argument is contradicted
by the evidence described earlier that demonstrated that hypnotic procedures
that were less leading than the one used with Bianchi led regularly to
enactments of hidden selves and past-life personalities in nonsimulating
college students (Spanos & Hewitt, 1980; Spanos, Menary, et al., 1991).
Hypnotic
interviews used to diagnose MPD are highly reminiscent of Catholic exorcism
procedures. They have, of course, been
secularised to meet the materialist assumptions of 20th century psychiatry, but
the major components are the same. The
hypnotist has replaced the priest, and he or she searches out and obtains the
names of alters rather than demons. The
priest "discovered" when the demons entered the body; the hypnotist
"discovers" when each alter split from the "birth
personality." The priest discovered why each demon entered the person; the
hypnotist discovers the supposed trauma that led to the formation of each
alter. The priest discovered the number
of indwelling demons; the hypnotist discovers the number of alters, and so
on. Exorcisms led regularly to the
production of multiple indwelling demons, and hypnotic interviews appear to
produce their secular counterparts: multiple indwelling personalities.
Motivations, Legitimation, and Multiple Personality
Disorder
Information
about MPD is widespread in North American culture, and the major components of
the role are now well known to the general public. Popular TV movies like Sybil and popular biographies like The Minds of Billy Milligan (Keyes, 1981) provide extensive
information about the symptoms of MPD, and MPD patients, along with their
psychiatrists, are sometimes even featured on popular TV talk shows. In all of these sources, MPD patients are
shown in an attractive light as people with dramatic symptoms who, with the
help of devoted and empathic therapists, surmount numerous obstacles to
eventually gain self-esteem, dignity, health, happiness, and much sympathetic
attention from high-status others. In
short, the idea of being a multiple, like the idea of suffering from peripheral
possession or demonic possession, may provide some people with a viable and
face-saving way to account for personal problems as well as a dramatic means
for gaining concern and attention from significant others. The role of the media in fostering MPD was
evident in a report by Gruenwald (1971) concerning a 17-year-old, hospitalised
female patient. This patient's first
enactment of an alter personality occurred the day after seeing the movie The Three Faces of Eve on
television. Relatedly, Fahy, Abas, and
Brown (1989) reported on a patient who presented symptoms of MPD and who had
seen the movie The Three Faces of Eve and
read the book Sybil (Schreiber,
1973).
Thigpen
and Cleckley (1957), the authors of The
Three Faces of Eve, commented on the attractions of an MPD diagnosis. After publication of their famous book, they
were frequently contacted by people who displayed symptoms similar to those
reported in the book for Eve and who sought out the authors to legitimate their
self-diagnosis of multiplicity:
“[Many of these patients] appeared to be
motivated (either consciously or unconsciously) by a desire to draw attention
to themselves. Certainly a diagnosis of
multiple personality attracts a good deal more attention than most other
diagnoses. Some patients appear to be
motivated by secondary gain associated with avoiding responsibility for certain
actions.” (Thigpen & Cleckley, 1984, p. 64)
Given
the attractions of the MPD diagnosis and the widespread knowledge of its
symptoms, it is not surprising that patients sometimes present such symptoms in
the absence of cueing from therapists.
Nevertheless, people are unlikely to sustain enactment of such a role in
the absence of legitimation. For
instance, the therapist who treated the case described by Fahy et al. (1989)
directed attention away from the patient's alters and focused on her other
problems in living. In the absence of
the therapist's legitimation, the patient's MPD enactments went into sharp
decline.
The
importance of interpersonal legitimation in the maintenance of alter identity
enactments was demonstrated by Kohlenberg (1973). Kohlenberg described how the psychiatric
staff of a ward that housed a multiple were sensitised to his three different
personality enactments and interacted with each personality in a different
manner. To demonstrate the importance of
contextual variables in maintaining multiple identity enactments, baseline
rates of occurrence were assessed for the behaviors associated with each of the
patient's three personalities.
Afterward, the behaviors associated with only one of the personalities
were selectively reinforced. The
behaviors associated with the reinforced personality increased dramatically in
frequency. In later extinction trials,
the frequency of occurrence of these behaviors decreased to baseline levels.
Currently,
the legitimation of MPD often involves a social dimension that transcends
patient-therapist interactions that occur in the consulting room. In some respects, advocacy of the MPD
diagnosis has taken on the characteristics of a social movement (Mulhern,
1991b, in press). MPD patients, along
with therapists, participate regularly in MPD workshops and conferences, and
both patients and therapists frequently have access to national newsletters
that provide updated information about the syndrome (Victor, 1993). Along with their individual therapy, many
patients participate in MPD self-help and therapy groups that provide ongoing
legitimation for their multiple self enactments. Patients in one highly vocal subset who
appear to enjoy their MPD status have even asserted their right to remain
multiples (Mulhern, 1991b, in press). Some
therapists employ MPD patients as co-therapists to help convince sceptical new
patients that their MPD diagnosis is accurate (Allison & Schwarz,
1980). Perry (1992) estimated that 17%
of the therapists treating MPD are themselves patients or former patients
diagnosed with MPD or other dissociative disorders (see also Mulhern, in
press). These therapists, who help to
socialise new patient recruits into the MPD role, are reminiscent of those in
traditional cultures who, after their own possession, join and sometimes become
leaders of the possession cults that shape and legitimate the spirit possession
enactments of new members.
In
summary, the importance of psychotherapy and therapy-related social supports in
the genesis and maintenance of MPD would be hard to overemphasise. Therapists are, after all, typically viewed
by their clients as competent experts whose opinions are highly valued and
whose suggestions are treated seriously.
In addition, psychotherapy clients are often insecure, unhappy people
with a strong investment in winning the concern, interest, and approval of their
therapist. This is likely to be
particularly true of the polysymptomatic, chronically disturbed women with a
long history of psychiatric involvement who are typically diagnosed as MPD
sufferers. Given these circumstances,
mutual shaping between therapists "on the lookout" for signs of MPD
and clients involved in creating an impression that will elicit approval is
likely to lead to enactments of multiple personality that confirm the initial
suspicions of the therapist and, in turn, lead the therapist to encourage and
validate more elaborate displays of the disorder (Sutcliffe & Jones,
1962). In addition, the newsletters,
therapy groups, workshops, and informal interactions with other multiples that
have become an important part of the social life of many MPD patients serve to
continually shape and legitimate multiple self enactments.
This
analysis does not imply that MPD patients are typically faking their
multiplicity. Instead, it suggests that
patients come to adopt a view of themselves that is congruent with the view
conveyed to them by their therapist.
Adoption of this view involves clients coming to construe their various
"symptoms" (e.g., mood swings, shameful or unrepresentative
behaviors, ambivalent feelings, hostile fantasies, forgetfulness, guilt-inducing
sexual feelings, and bad habits) as the results of personified alter
selves. In North American culture, it is
common for people to describe uncharacteristic or ambivalent feelings and
behaviors metaphorically as resulting from different parts of themselves (e.g.,
"One part of me wanted to do it but another part said no" or
"I'm of two minds about the issue").
A socio-cognitive analysis suggests that the development of MPD involves
a reification of such metaphors that leads both the client and the therapist to
construe the client as possessing multiple selves (Sarbin, in press).
Child Abuse and Multiple Personality
Disorder
Most
studies find that MPD patients report extremely high rates of childhood sexual
or physical abuse, or both (e.g., Coons et al., 1988; Ross, Miller, et al., 199
1; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, 1989).
These findings are the major source of empirical support for the
hypothesis that MPD results from early trauma.
As described earlier, however, data obtained from North American spirit
mediums, as well as from experimental subjects who report past-life identities,
indicate that multiplicity can occur in the absence of early child abuse. In addition, the correlational nature of the
MPD/child abuse findings precludes their establishing a causal relationship
between abuse and MPD. Moreover, the
frequent reports of child abuse from MPD patients can be accounted for without
positing that abuse causes MPD.
The
sexual abuse of children appears to be a relatively common occurrence in North
American culture, and such abuse occurs more frequently in girls than in boys
(Finkelhor, 1979). For instance,
depending on the criteria for defining abuse and the characteristics of the
samples assessed, rates of reported childhood sexual abuse in women ranged from
a low of 27% to a high of 62% in a series of studies reviewed by Pope and
Hudson (1992). Even higher rates have
been reported in some clinical samples.
Briere and Zaidi (1989) found that 70% of 50 consecutive female patients
visiting an urban emergency room reported a history of child sexual abuse. The short-term and long-term psychological
effects of child sexual abuse are by no means clear because of serious
methodological problems in the studies that have addressed these issues (Beitchman,
Zucker, Hood, DaCosta, & Akman, 1991; Beitchman et al., 1992). Nevertheless, people who were abused as
children often report a range of psychiatric symptoms and undoubtedly come to
the attention of mental health professionals at least as frequently as the
nonabused. Consequently, the high rate
of abuse in MPD patients can be partly explained by the fact that a substantial
proportion of chronically disturbed people who seek psychiatric help
(particularly women) are likely to report a history of child abuse. In addition, because some clinicians consider
a history of abuse to be a possible sign of MPD, they may be more likely to
expose abused than nonabused patients to leading hypnotic interviews and other "diagnostic"
procedures that generate displays of multiplicity. To the extent that this occurs, the idea that
early abuse is associated with MPD becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Some
patients who receive an MPD diagnosis do not remember having been abused in
childhood until their multiplicity is "discovered" in therapy. In these cases, the patient's alter
personalities report abuse that had purportedly been dissociated from the
memory of the presenting personality.
Under these circumstances, there is reason to believe that such newly
"remembered" abuse is often fabricated. In other words, patients who develop the
expectation that they must have been abused may construct fantasies of such
abuse. Given their expectations and the
validation of these fantasies as memories by their therapists, the fantasies
are experienced as real memories of abuse (Loftus, 1993b; Spanos, Burgess,
& Burgess, in press).
According
to MPD proponents, abuse that occurs during childhood is often so traumatic
that it is forgotten (i.e., dissociated from the primary personality). This hypothesis predicts that children who
have suffered documented psychological trauma will later be unable to recall
that trauma. However, the available data
gathered in prospective studies of traumatised children suggest instead that
children who are more than 4 or 5 years of age at the time that they are
traumatised usually remember rather than forget the traumatic events years
after their occurrence (Femina, Yeager, & Lewis, 1990; Terr, 1988, 1990). Femina et al. (1990) found that people who
were abused as children sometimes denied the abuse when interviewed years
later. In a second interview, however,
these subjects invariably acknowledged remembering the abuse and described
their earlier denials as due to factors such as embarrassment about what had
occurred rather than memory loss.
Obviously, the findings of these studies do not demonstrate that people
never forget early abuse. Nevertheless,
they suggest that reports of previously forgotten childhood trauma that emerge
for the first time during therapy should be treated cautiously rather than
accepted as accurate descriptions of early events.
It
is now generally acknowledged that memory involves reconstructive elements and
is strongly influenced by current beliefs and expectations (Friedman, 1993;
Loftus, 1979). People typically organise
their recall of past events in a way that makes sense of their present
situation and is congruent with their current expectations. The specifics of what they recall are
frequently in error and involve a mixture of correctly remembered and
misremembered information that is often impossible to disentangle. Frequently, there is little or no correlation
between the accuracy of recall and the confidence that people place in their
recall. It is not unusual for people to
be convinced about the accuracy of a remembrance that turns out to be false
(Loftus, 1979; Wells, Ferguson, & Lindsay, 1981). In addition, leading questions and other
suggestive interview procedures can produce a substantial deterioration in
recall accuracy even when subjects remain highly confident of their inaccurate
remembrances (Spanos, Gwynn, et al., 1989).
Contrary to popular belief, hypnotic procedures do not reliably enhance
the accuracy of recall and, at least under some circumstances, may lead
subjects to become even more overconfident than usual in their inaccurate
recall (Smith, 1983; Spanos, Quigley, Gwynn, Glatt, & Perlini, 1991). The fallibility of memory should be kept in mind
when examining reports of child abuse that emerge for the first time during
therapy (Loftus, 1993b; Wakefield & Underwager, 1992).
The
strong connection between child abuse and MPD is of recent origin. Cases reported in the early part of the 20th
century and before were much less likely than modern cases to be associated
with reports of child abuse (Bowman, 1990; Kenny, 1986). Moreover, the abuse that was reported in
these early cases lacked the lurid ritualistic elements that are becoming an
increasingly prominent characteristic of the abuse memories proffered by modem
MPD patients. An association between MPD
and child abuse first came to prominence in the 1970s with the concurrent rise
in public interest in child sexual abuse.
In the early 1970s, the book Sybil
(Schreiber, 1973) described the sadistic childhood abuse purportedly
suffered by an MPD patient. The book and
later television movie were exceedingly popular and Sybil, even more than Eve
(Thigpen & Cleckley, 1957) in the previous decade, became a model of the
MPD survivor that greatly influenced the expectations of therapists and
patients alike (Putnam, 1989).
Consequently, when MPD patients claim that they cannot remember having
been abused, therapists tend to disbelieve them (Bliss, 1986). Instead, therapists may prod them repeatedly
in an attempt to unearth such memories.
When patients believe they may be fantasizing rather than remembering
abuse, their uncertainty may be presented to them as evidence that they are
unwilling to face the fact of their abuse (Bliss, 1986). In short, MPD therapists appear to frequently
use leading and suggestive procedures to elicit abuse memories from their
patients (Mulhern, 1991a).
A
number of years after the publication of Sybil,
a book titled Michelle Remembers (Smith & Pazder, 1980) reported on ritual
satanic tortures that a woman had purportedly experienced during childhood and
then forgotten until they were recovered during therapy. Michelle's story became a part of the
propaganda used by the evangelical Christian movement that became increasingly
prominent in many facets of American social and political life during the
1980s. This movement reinvigorated the
mythology of Satanism. Like its 16th-
and 17th-century predecessor, this reinvigorated mythology holds that there
exists a powerful but secret international satanic conspiracy that carries out
heinous crimes. These crimes supposedly
include the kidnapping, torture, and sexual abuse of countless children as well
as murder, forced pregnancies, and cannibalism (Bromley, 1991; Hicks, 1991;
Lyons, 1988).
Large
numbers of therapists who identified themselves as active Christians joined the
MPD movement in the 1980s (Mulhern, in press), and soon accounts like those of
Michelle began to be reported by the alters of MPD patients (Fraser, 1990;
Young et al., 1991). By the mid- 1980s,
25% of MPD patients in therapy had recovered memories of ritual satanic abuse,
and, by 1992, the percentage of patients recovering such memories was as high
as 80% in some treatment facilities (Mulhern, in press).
If
the ritual satanic crimes "remembered" by MPD patients actually
occurred, it would necessitate the existence of a monumental criminal
conspiracy that has been in existence for at least 50 years and has been
responsible for the murder of thousands of people (Hicks, 1991). Law enforcement agencies throughout North
America have investigated numerous allegations of satanic abuse made by MPD
patients but have been unable to substantiate the existence of the requisite
criminal conspiracy (Lanning, 1989). It
is important to understand that criminal conspiracies are very difficult to
hide. This, of course, is particularly
true when large numbers of conspirators are involved and the crimes include
murder and cannibalism, which leave physical evidence that is difficult to
eliminate (Lanning, 1989). Thus, the
repeated failure of law enforcement agencies to obtain support for the satanic
abuse allegations of MPD patients constitutes strong evidence that the vast
majority of these allegations are false and that the "memories" on
which they are based are fantasies rather than remembrances of actual events
(Hicks, 1991). Like the satanic
conspiracy of the 16th and 17th centuries, the modern conspiracy exists only in
the imaginations of its propagators.
Recently,
Bottoms, Shaver, and Goodman (1991) surveyed psychologists across the United
States about the frequency with which they had seen patients who reported
ritual abuse memories. Seventy percent
of the respondents reported that they had never seen such patients. A small minority, however, reported having
seen large numbers of patients who reported ritual abuse. This pattern of findings suggests that
therapists who obtain such reports regularly may play an active role in shaping
the ritual abuse memories of their patients.
Mulhern
(1991a) described the hypnotic procedures sometimes used to elicit satanic
ritual abuse memories in MPD patients:
“During hypnotic interviews clinicians
explicitly described satanic ritual scenes or displayed pictures of satanic
symbols to a patient; then addressed "all parts of the patient's
mind" or "everyone inside," requesting that any part who
recognised the satanic material so indicate either by a nod of the head or by
prearranged yes, no and stop ideomotor signals . . . is it possible that these
clinicians never paused to consider just what kind of message a patient would
receive from a clinician who is holding up snapshots and asking if the patient
can identify people as leaders of a group of cannibalistic devil worshippers.”
(p. 610)
The
importance of leading interrogations in obtaining memories of ritual abuse was
underscored in the case of a suspect described by Ofshe (1992) who confessed to
raping and ritually abusing his own children as part of a satanic cult. None of the satanic elements of his story
could be confirmed by police investigation, and many of the events to which he
confessed were suggested to him by the police officers and psychologist who
interrogated him. The events to which he
confessed were legitimised as memories rather than fantasies by his
fundamentalist minister. Ofshe
interrogated the suspect with the leading questions and guided fantasy
procedures used by the police and suggested to him that he had committed a set
of abuse events that the police agreed had not occurred. The suspect readily confessed to the false
events and, after the interrogation, insisted that those events had really
occurred. Although the suspect had no
psychiatric history before his arrest, he was diagnosed by one psychologist
involved in the case as suffering from MPD.
Whitley
(1992) described several former patients from the same psychiatric clinic who
reported ritual abuse memories during their therapy but later disavowed the
reality of these reports. The patients
(some of whom were diagnosed as multiples) described their reports of early
abuse as fantasies that were suggested and encouraged by their therapists and
by copatients in psychotherapy groups.
These patients reported a great deal of interpersonal pressure to
generate reports of severe abuse and to define those reports as memories as
opposed to fantasies.
Some
patients report memory fragments or dreams with satanic content and only
afterward are exposed to hypnotic interviews aimed at confirming such abuse. However, because many MPD patients are
enmeshed in a social network in which they hear about satanic abuse from other
patients, therapists, and shared newsletters and in which they or their fellow
patients attend workshops devoted to such abuse, "spontaneous" dreams
and memories of this kind are hardly surprising and do not provide serious
evidence of actual ritual abuse. In this
context, it is worth recalling the ease with which highly hypnotisable college
students were induced to enact past-life personalities who
"remembered" that they had been abused as children when the
expectation of such abuse had been conveyed to them before their hypnotic
regression (Spanos, Menary, et al., 1991).
Also relevant are the reports of glossolalics in some congregations and
spirit mediums in some cultures who reorganise their biographies to bring them
into line with cultural expectations concerning the past histories of possessed
people.
The
reporting of ritual satanic abuse is not the only unusual characteristic of the
child abuse reported by MPD patients.
For instance, some evidence suggests that the populations from which
subjects are selected influence the incidence with which MPD patients report
child abuse histories. Ross, Norton, and
Fraser (1989) found that a sample of American psychiatrists reported a much
higher prevalence of child sexual abuse in their MPD patients (81.2%) than did
Canadian psychiatrists (45.5%). Relatedly, Ross (1991) reported that subjects
drawn from a non-clinical population and diagnosed with MPD reported much lower
rates of child abuse than MPD patients drawn from clinical populations. The findings of these studies challenge the
idea of a causal relationship between child abuse and MPD.
Studies
on the sexual abuse of children (reviewed by Wakefield & Underwager, 1991)
indicate that the abusers are very unlikely to be female and that this is
particularly true when the victim is female.
Some studies of hospitalised adolescents who had been sexually abused as
children reported that the perpetrators were never female (e.g.,
Sansonnet-Hayden, Haley, Marriage, & Fine, 1987). Given the consistency of these findings,
those reported by Ross, Miller, et al. (1 99 1) for MPD patients raise suspicions
about validity. These investigators reported
that 15.7% of their MPD patients had been sexually abused by their mothers;
2.9%, 10.8%, and 21.6%, respectively, had been so abused by their stepmothers,
other female relatives, and other females.
Unfortunately, Ross, Miller, et al. (1991) did not provide the total
percentage of patients abused by a female; however, that total obviously must
have been quite substantial and well above what would be expected on the basis
of other studies of child abuse. Ross,
Miller, et al. (1991) also reported on the age of earliest sexual abuse for
their subjects. A substantial 26.6%
reported being sexually abused before 3 years of age, and 10.6% reported being
sexually abused before their first birthday.
The fact that these ages are much younger than the age at which abuse
typically begins (see Wakefield & Underwager, 1992) is problematic. More troubling is that these data were based
entirely on the retrospective reports of the patients. Studies on recall of early memories have
usually indicated that people are unable to recall events that occurred before
3 years of age, and these studies have reported uniformly that subjects are
unable to recall events before their first birthday (Howe & Courage, 1993;
Loftus, 1993a). Ross, Miller, et al.
(1991) said nothing about corroboration of their patients' early memories, and
one is left wondering how these patients were able to remember what happened to
them before the age of I or 2 years.
In
summary, the high rates of child abuse reported by MPD patients do not constitute
good evidence that such abuse causes multiplicity. A number of different non-causal factors
probably contribute to the high rates of child abuse reported by these patients
(e.g., high base rates for abuse in chronic psychiatric populations, the use of
abuse histories as a criterion for conducting MPD-eliciting hypnotic
interviews, and treatment-induced fabrication).
No doubt, many people who become MPD patients were abused during
childhood. Nevertheless, most people who
suffer even severe child abuse do not exhibit MPD, and many people who have not
been abused can easily and quickly be induced to display multiplicity (e.g.,
college students given past-life regression suggestions and mentally healthy
spirit mediums). Taken together, these
findings argue against a causal relationship between child abuse and later
displays of multiplicity.
Hypnotizability and Multiple Personality
Disorder
MPD
patients frequently attain high scores on standardised hypnotizability scales
(Bliss, 1980, 1986; Bliss & Larson, 1985).
The suggestions included on these scales are communications that call
for the construction of "as if" situations (Bertrand, 1989). Thus, suggestions for arm levitation,
hallucination, and amnesia are, in effect, tacit requests to use imaginative
and other cognitive abilities to behave as if one's arm is rising by itself, as
if one is unable to remember, and so forth.
Behaving as if an imaginary scenario is true involves creating the
requisite subjective experiences as well as generating the requisite behaviors
(Sarbin & Coe, 1972; Spanos, Rivers, & Ross, 1977). The hypnotic interviews typically used to
"call up" alter personalities, like the standardised suggestions on
hypnotizability scales, also include requests to construct an as if scenario:
to behave and experience as if one has alter identities.
MPD
proponents interpret the finding that MPD patients frequently attain high
hypnotizability to mean that hypnotizability scores reflect individual
differences in the capacity for dissociation (Putnam, 1993). This hypothesis predicts a substantial
correlation between hypnotizability and independent indexes of
dissociation. Stava and Jaffa (1988)
developed several objective indexes of dissociative capacity (e.g., degree of
success at dividing attention and performing two tasks simultaneously). None of these correlated significantly with
hypnotizability. Bernstein and Putnam (1
986) developed a questionnaire index of dissociation, and Putnam (1989)
reported that it correlated significantly with hypnotizability. However, two studies (Nadon, Hoyt, Register,
& Kihlstrom, 1991; Spanos, Arango, & de Groot, 1993) found that
dissociation scores correlated significantly with hypnotizability only when both
indexes were assessed in the same context.
When dissociation was assessed in a context that subjects did not
associate with their hypnotizability testing, it failed to correlate
significantly with hypnotizability.
These findings indicate a lack of any intrinsic relationship between
dissociation scores and hypnotizability.
Whether or not these dimensions are found to be related appears to be
dependent on expectations generated by testing both dimensions in the same
context.
Most
studies report no differences between the sexes in hypnotizability (for a
review, see deGroot, 1989). In North
American studies, however, women are assigned the MPD diagnosis much more
frequently than men, whereas the one Swiss study (Modestin, 1992) found that
men were assigned the diagnosis slightly more frequently than women. These findings indicate that the relationship
between MPD and hypnotizability must be moderated by other variables. The hypothesis that this moderator is early
child abuse requires that child abuse occur at least three times more
frequently in girls than in boys in North America but with about equal
frequency in boys and girls in Switzerland.
This hypothesis further suggests that the combination of high
hypnotizability and early child abuse is much rarer in Great Britain and in
other societies with very low rates of MPD than it is in North America. The available evidence does not indicate that
high hypnotizability is more common in North America than in Great Britain, nor
does the evidence indicate very low rates of child abuse in Great Britain (La
Fontaine, 1990).
An
alternative hypothesis holds that high hypnotizables are particularly likely to
respond with displays of multiplicity to the cueing contained in leading
interviews. This hypothesis holds that
hypnotizability is a stable, trait-like index of suggestibility and that high
hypnotizables constitute only about 10% of the population. Because high hypnotizables are more
suggestible than moderate or low hypnotizables, they are particularly likely to
respond to the strong cueing for MPD contained in leading diagnostic interviews
(Ganaway, 1989; Mulhern, 1991b).
This
suggestibility trait hypothesis accounts for why MPD patients typically score
as high hypnotizables. Moreover, it can
account for different cross-national gender ratios and incidence rates by
suggesting that they reflect the expectations of therapists who use leading
hypnotic procedures. Those who believe
that MPD is relatively common will use such procedures frequently, those who
believe that it is more common in women than men will use such procedures more
frequently with women than with men, and so on.
However, the suggestibility trait hypothesis cannot account
parsimoniously for the very high rates of multiplicity seen in some traditional
cultures (e.g., 47% of the women in some villages) or for the very high rates
of demonic possession seen in some group possession cases (e.g., the case in
which all or almost all of the nuns in a particular convent displayed
possession simultaneously; Huxley, 1952).
This hypothesis also has difficulty with the findings that glossolalics
do not exhibit particularly high hypnotizability (Spanos & Hewitt, 1979)
and that, in some congregations, glossolalia occurs in all or almost all
members (Goodman, 1972).
An
alternative to the suggestibility trait hypothesis holds that a great many
people who do not typically attain high hypnotizability scores possess the
cognitive abilities required to enact the as if scenarios contained in hypnotic
suggestions and in hypnotic interviews that call for multiplicity (Spanos,
1986a). Along these lines, a number of
recent studies have demonstrated that short training procedures aimed at
altering subjects' interpretations and attitudes produce large gains in
hypnotizability (e.g., Gorassini, Sowerby, Creighton, & Fry, 1991; Spanos,
Gabora, Jarrett, & Gwynn, 1989; Spanos, Lush, & Gwynn, 1989). Thus, a much greater proportion of people
than indicated by conventional hypnotizability testing possess the cognitive
abilities required to experience the as if scenarios traditionally associated
with hypnotic suggestions.
The
leading interviews used to diagnose MPD are frequently conducted in a hypnotic
context, and the psychotherapeutic procedures used with these patients almost
always make use of hypnotic procedures.
In other words, almost all MPD patients have been repeatedly
administered hypnotic procedures and have responded repeatedly to these
procedures by displaying alter personalities.
Given this, MPD patients are very likely to construe themselves as
highly hypnotisable and are likely to be motivated to respond to communications
delivered in a hypnotic context.
Consequently, when they are tested for hypnotizability, they tend to
respond in terms of the motivations and expectations derived from their earlier
hypnotic experiences and, therefore, attain high hypnotizability scores.
This
contextualist hypothesis does not deny that individual differences on some
cognitive or interactional style dimensions may influence the ease with which
people carry out multiple identity enactments.
For example, some clinical reports describe MPD patients as highly
imaginative people with rich fantasy lives who have spent much time covertly
rehearsing and becoming absorbed in a range of "make believe"
activities (e.g., Keyes, 1981). Given
appropriate motivations, it is easy to understand that such people might be
particularly adept at enacting multiple identities when contextual inducements
call for such enactments.
Relatedly,
some clinical studies describe many MPD patients as exhibiting symptoms of
borderline personality disorder (e.g., Horevitz & Braun, 1984). Borderlines are described as displaying
sudden mood shifts; rapid, marked changes in attitude; impulsivity; and sudden,
inappropriate anger. When provided with
the idea of MPD, such people may find it particularly easy to conceptualise
their rapid shifts in mood and behavior as stemming from different selves at
war with one another.
Many
MPD patients are diagnosed as psychopaths at some point in their careers (Bliss,
1986). In short, they are often adept at
altering their self-presentations to manipulate others. Given that they see it in their best
interests, such people may be particularly adept at enacting multiplicity.
In
summary, there may be numerous cognitive propensities and interactional styles
that influence the ease with which individuals carry out multiplicity
enactments. On the other hand,
motivational and contextual demand variables will undoubtedly interact with and
may at times even override the effects of such individual difference
variables. In addition, it is important
to keep in mind that individual difference variables that might facilitate
multiple identity enactments in one cultural context may be unimportant or even
a hindrance to such enactments in other cultures. For example, borderline characteristics might
facilitate a self-definition of multiplicity when multiplicity is defined as a
pathology and when erratic and unpredictable behaviors are congruent with the
multiple identity role. However, the
same characteristics might well hinder displays of multiplicity in a shaman who
is required to display restraint and good judgement and whose multiple identity
enactments entail regular conformance with ritual requirements.
In
summary, cross-cultural differences in the rates with which multiplicity is
displayed are very large, and in some cultures substantial proportions of the
population display multiplicity at some point in their lives. These considerations suggest that the
cognitive abilities required to experience and enact multiplicity are fairly
common and that the frequency of such enactments is limited as much, and
perhaps more, by contextual considerations (e.g., the practices of different
therapists and the opportunity to observe more practised demoniacs) as by
stable individual differences in cognitive abilities.
Conclusions
Multiple
identity enactments occur in most human societies. Nevertheless, the frequency of these
enactments, their behavioural components, the conditions under which they
occur, and the characteristics of those who display them differ dramatically
between cultures and sometimes within cultures as well. When examined across cultures and historical
eras, the rule-governed nature of multiple identity enactments and their
embeddedness within a legitimising social matrix become clear.
Each
culture develops its own indigenous theory of multiple identity
enactments. These local theories reflect
local social structures and institutions, and they translate into culturally
specific expectations that guide both the performance of multiple identity
enactments and the reactions of audiences to these enactments. Thus, the theory that the Holy Ghost speaks
through people and, in so doing, endorses them spiritually provides a local
explanation for glossolalia and helps to establish motivations and expectations
for its perpetuation. However, local
theories of multiplicity are designed to explain only local displays, and their
deficiencies as general theories of multiplicity become obvious when these
enactments are compared across historical and cultural contexts.
The
disease theory of MPD is a local theory.
Because the proponents of this theory are invariably mental health
professionals, the displays of multiplicity that they observe are usually
limited to those distressed people who go to them for help. This limitation profoundly influences the
manner in which these investigators conceptualise multiplicity. The disease theory of MPD is based on the
idea that unhappiness or behavioural deviance in adulthood stems from
particular traumatic events occurring in childhood. The particular childhood traumas on which
modern MPD proponents focus are physical abuse and, especially, sexual
abuse. Because of its emphasis on childhood
antecedents and on the notion that "symptoms" reflect unconscious
defences, this approach tends to greatly de-emphasize the social and strategic
nature of multiple identity enactments and the roles played by the
institutionalised contexts that encourage, shape, and legitimise these
enactments. In particular, this emphasis
deflects attention away from the role clinicians themselves play in cueing and
legitimising manifestations of multiplicity.
It also deflects attention from the marked changes in symptomatology
that have occurred in MPD over the years, changes that illustrate the role of
social factors in shaping MPD displays.
Since the 19th century, for example, the number of personalities per
patient has jumped from 2 or 3 to often more than 20 and sometimes into the
hundreds. Early cases were marked by
transitional periods of sleep and convulsions, which are uncommon today. The alters of early patients were human. Recently, however, scholarly articles have
been devoted to animal alters (e.g., Hendrickson, McCarty, & Goodwin,
1990), and the alters "discovered" by some clinicians now exhibit the
characteristics of semi-spiritual self helpers (R. B. Allison & Schwarz,
1980) or reincarnated past lives (Krippner, 1986). Reports of child abuse have gone from
occasional accompaniments of early cases to the ritual satanic abuse of today.
Changes
of these kinds are difficult to deal with from a perspective that explains
identity enactments as symptoms caused by past traumas rather than as
expectancy-guided displays that change with new information concerning role
demands. As indicated earlier, role
changes of this kind are commonly seen in historical and cross-cultural
displays of possession. Songhay mediums,
for example, changed the characteristics of their possession displays in
response to changes among the ruling elites (Stoller, 1989), and the
characteristics of demonic possession changed to meet the different
requirements of particular religious communities (Spanos, 1983a; Walker, 1981). Relatedly, the recent association of MPD with
reports of ritual satanic abuse is much more likely to reflect therapy-induced
fabrications generated by the infusion of evangelical Christian ideology into
the MPD movement than the existence of a 50-year-old secret conspiracy that has
been responsible for the murder of thousands without leaving a trace of
evidence.
Like
other local theories, the disease theory fails to provide a general account of
multiplicity that takes into consideration its cross-cultural and transhistorical
manifestations. I suggest that the local
theory of MPD be abandoned and the phenomenon of multiplicity viewed from a
socio-cognitive and historical perspective.
References
Ackerman, S. E., & Lee, R. L. M.
(1981). Communication and cognitive
pluralism in a spirit possession event in Malaysia. American
Ethnologist, 8, 789-799.
Adityhanjee, R., & Khandelwal, S.
(1989). Current status of multiple
personality disorder in India. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1607-1610.
Aldridge-Morris, R. (1989). Multiple
personality.. An exercise in deception. Hilisdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Allison, R. B. (1978). On discovering multiplicity. Swedish
Journal of Hypnosis, 2, 4-8.
Allison, R. B., & Schwarz, T. (1980).
Minds in many pieces. New
York: Rawson Wade.
Allison, R. M. (1991). Travel log: In search of multiples in Moscow.
American Journal of Forensic Psychiatry,
12, 51-56.
Altroechi, J. (1992). "We don't have that problem here":
MPD in New Zealand. Dissociation, 5, 109-1 10.
Baker, R. A. (1992). Hidden
memories. Buffalo, NY. Prometheus.
Barber, T. X. (1969). Hypnosis:
A scientific approach. New York: Van
Nostrand Reinhold.
Barber, T. X. (1979). Suggested ("hypnotic") behavior:
The trance paradigm versus an alternative paradigm. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research and new
perspectives (pp. 217-271). Chicago:
Aldine.
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J.
E., DaCosta, G. A., & Akman, D. (1991).
A review of short-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 15, 537-556.
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J.
E., DaCosta, G. A., Akman, D., & Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of long-term effects of child sexual
abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 101-118.
Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W.
(1986). Development, reliability, and
validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 174,
727-734.
Bertrand, L. D. (1989). The assessment and modification of hypnotic
susceptibility. In N. P. Spanos & J.
E Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The
cognitive-behavioural perspective (pp. 18-31). Buffalo, NY.
Prometheus.
Biscop, P. (1981). By spirit possessed. Unpublished master's thesis, Department of
Anthropology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada.
Biscop, P. (1985). There
is no death: Belief and the social construction of reality in a Canadian
spiritualist church. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Simon Fraser University,
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada.
Bliss, E. L. (1980). Multiple personalities: A report of 14 cases
with implications for schizophrenia and hysteria. Archives
of General Psychiatry, 37, 1388-1397.
Bliss, E. L. (1986). Multiple personality, allied
disorders and hypnosis. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bliss, E. L., & Jeppsen, E. A.
(1985). Prevalence of multiple
personality among inpatients and outpatients.
American Journal of Psychiatry,
142, 250-251.
Bliss, E. L., & Larson, E. M.
(1985). Sexual criminality and
hypnotizability. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 522-526.
Bliss, E. L., Larson, E. M., &
Nakashima, S. R. (1983). Auditory
hallucinations and schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 30-33.
Boddy, J. (1988). Spirits and selves in northern Sudan: The
cultural therapeutics of possession and trance.
American Ethnologist, 15, 427.
Bottoms, B. L., Shaver, P. R., &
Goodman, G. S. (1991, August). Profile of ritual and religion related abuse
allegations reported to clinical psychologists in the United States. Paper presented at the 99th Annual
Convention of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA.
Bourguignon, E. (1976). Possession. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.
Bowman, E. S. (1990). Adolescent multiple personality disorder in
the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries.
Dissociation, 3, 179-187.
Brandsma, J. M., & Ludwig, A. M.
(1974). A case of multiple personality:
Diagnosis and treatment. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 22, 216-233.
Braun, B. G. (1984). Hypnosis creates multiple personality: Myth
or reality? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 32, 191-197.
Braun, B. G. (1990). Multiple personality disorder: An
overview. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 44, 971-976.
Briere, J., & Zaidi, L. Y. (1989). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in female
psychiatric emergency room patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1602~1607.
Bromley, D. G. (1 99 1). Satanism: The new cult scare. In J. T
Richardson, J. Best, & D. G. Bromley (Eds.), The Satanism scare (pp. 49-72).
Chicago: Aldine.
Brown, P. (in press). Toward a psychobiological study of
dissociation. In S. J. Lynn (Ed.), Dissociation: Clinical, theoretical and
research perspectives. Washington,
DC: Guilford Press.
Burgess, C. A., Spanos, N. P., Ritt, J.,
Hordy, T, & Brooks, S. (1993). Compliant
responding among high hypnotizables: An experimental demonstration. Unpublished manuscript, Carleton
University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
Carstairs, G, M., & Kapur, R. L.
(1976). The great universe of Kota. London: Hogarth Press.
Chordoff, P. (1987). More on multiple personality disorder [letter
to the editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry 144, 124.
Chu, J. A., & Dill, D. L. (1990). Dissociative symptoms in relation to
childhood sexual abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 887-892.
Coe, W. C. (1989). Posthypnotic amnesia: Theory and
research. In N. P. Spanos & J. F.
Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis.. The
cognitive-behavioural perspective (pp. 418-436). Buffalo, NY.
Prometheus.
Coe, W. C., & Sluis, A. S. E.
(1989). Increasing contextual pressures
to breach posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57. 885-894.
Cohn, N. (1975). Europe's inner demons. New York: Basic Books.
Constantinides, P. (1977). Ill at ease and sick at heart: Symbolic
behavior in a Sudanese healing cult. In
1. Wilson (Ed.), Symbols and sentiments (pp.
61-84). NY.. Academic Press.
Coons, P. M., Bowman, E. S., & Milstein,
V. (1988). Multiple personality
disorder: A clinical investigation of 50 cases. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 176, 519-527.
Coons, P. M., & Milstein, V (1986). Psychosexual disturbances in multiple
personality: Characteristics, etiology, and treatment. Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry, 47, 106-110.
Council, J. R. (1993a, June). The annoying itch of context effects. Paper presented at the Nags Head
Conference on Personality and Social Behavior, Highland Beach, FL.
Council, J. R. (1993b). Context effects in personality research. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 2. 31-34.
Deaux, K. (1993). Reconstructing social identity. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 4-12.
deGroh, M. (1989). Correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. In N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioural
perspective (pp. 32-63). Buffalo,
NY.. Prometheus.
Diamond, M. J. (1974). Modification of hypnotizability: A
review. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 180-198.
DiTomasso, M. J., & Routh, D. K.
(1993). Recall of abuse in childhood and
three measures of dissociation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 477-485.
Docter, R. F. (1988). Transvestites and
transsexuals: Toward a theory of cross-gender behavior New York: Plenum.
Epstein, S. (1973). The self concept revisited: Or a theory of a
theory. American Psychologist, 28, 404-416.
Fahy, T. A. (1988). The diagnosis of multiple personality
disorder: A critical review. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 597~606.
Fahy, T. A., Abas, M., & Brown, J. C.
(1989). Multiple personality. A symptom of psychiatric disorder. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 99-101.
Fellows, B. (1986). The concept of trance. In P. L. N. Naish (Ed.), What is hypnosis? (pp. 37-58). Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Femina, D. D., Yeager, C. A., & Lewis,
D. O. (1990). Child abuse: Adolescent
records vs. adult recall. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14, 227-231.
Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually
victimised children. New York: Free
Press.
Firth, R. (1967). Ritual and drama in Malay spirit
mediumship. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 9, 190-207.
Franklin, J. (1988). Diagnosis of covert and subtle forms of
multiple personality disorder. Dissociation, 2, 27~33.
Fraser, G. A. (1990). Satanic ritual abuse: A cause of multiple
personality disorder. Journal of Child and Youth Care, Special
Issue, 55-66.
Friedman, W. (1 993). Memory for the time of past events. Psychological
Bulletin, 113, 44-66.
Galanter, M. (1990). Cults and zealous self-help movements: A
psychiatric perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 543-551.
Ganaway, G. K. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying
the role of erogenous trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants. Dissociation, 2, 205-220.
Goffman, E. ( 1959). The presentation of self in
everyday life. New York: Doubleday.
Goodman, F. D. (1972). Speaking in tongues: A
cross-cultural study of glossolalia. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Gorassini, D. R., Sowerby, D., Creighton,
A., & Fry, G. (1991). Hypnotic
suggestibility enhancement through brief cognitive skill training.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 289-297.
Gruenwald, D. (1971). Hypnotic techniques without hypnosis in the
treatment of a dual personality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 153, 41-46.
Hacking, 1. (1986). The invention of split personalities. In A. Donagan, A. N. Perovich, Jr., & M.
V. Wedin (Eds.), Human nature and natural
knowledge (pp. 63-85). New York: D.
Reidel.
Hacking, 1. (1991). Double consciousness in Britain
1815-1875. Dissociation, 4, 134-146.
Hacking, I. (1992). Multiple personality and its hosts. History
of the Human Sciences, 5, 3-3 1.
Halleck, S. L. (1990). Dissociative phenomena and the question of
responsibility. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 38, 298-314.
Harper, E. B. (1963). Spirit possession and social structure. In B. Ratman (Ed.), Anthropology on the march. (pp. 56-62) London: Oxford University
Press.
Harsnett, S. (1599). Discovery of the fraudulent
practices of John Darrel. London: John Wolfe.
Harsnett, S. (1603). A
declaration of egregious popish impostures. London: I. Roberts.
Hendrickson, K. M., McCarty, T., &
Goodwin, J. M. (1990). Animal alters:
Case reports. Dissociation, 4, 218-221.
Hicks, R. D. (1991). In
pursuit of Satan. Buffalo, NY..
Prometheus.
Hilgard, E. R. (1979). Divided consciousness in hypnosis: The
implications of the hidden observer. In
E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis:
Developments in research and new perspectives (pp. 45-79). Chicago: Aldine.
Hilgard, E. R. (1991). A neodissociation interpretation of
hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. Rhue
(Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current
models and perspectives (pp. 83-104).
New York: Guilford Press.
Hilgard, J. R. (1970). Personality and hypnosis: A study of imaginative
involvement. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Hine, V. H. (1969). Pentecostal glossolalia: Toward a functional
reinterpretation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 8, 212-226.
Hine, V. H. (1970). Bridge burners: Commitment and participation
in a religious movement. Sociological Analysis, 31, 61-66.
Horevitz, R. P., & Braun, B. G.
(1984). Are multiple personalities
borderlines? An analysis of 33
cases. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 7, 69-87.
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L.
(1993). On resolving the enigma of
infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305~326.
Huxley, A. (1952). The
devils of Loudon. New York: Harper
& Row.
Kampman, R. (1976). Hypnotically induced multiple personality: An
experimental study. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 24, 215-227.
Keene, M. L. (1976). The
psychic Mafia. New York: Dell.
Kelly, H. A. (1974). The
devil, demonology and witchcraft. New
York: Doubleday.
Kenny, M. G. (1986). The passion of Ansel Bourne..
Multiple personality and American culture.
Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Keyes, D. (1981). The minds of Billy
Milligan. New York: Bantam Books.
Kirshner, L. A. (1973). Dissociative reactions: An historical review
and clinical study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 49, 698-711.
Kluft, R. P. (1982). Varieties of hypnotic interventions in the
treatment of multiple personality. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 24, 230~240.
Kluft, R. P. (1 985). The natural history of multiple personality
disorder. In R. P. Kluft (Ed.), Childhood
antecedents of multiple personality (pp. 197-238). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Kluft, R. P. (1993). Multiple personality disorders. In D. Spiegel (Ed.), Dissociative disorders.. A clinical review (pp. 17-44). Lutherville, MD: Sidran.
Kohlenberg, R. J. (1973). Behavioristic approach to multiple
personality: A case study. Behavior Therapy, 4, 137-140.
Krippner, S. (1986). Cross-cultural approaches to multiple
personality disorder: Therapeutic practices in Brazilian spiritism. Humanistic
Psychologist, 14, 177-193.
Krippner, S. (1989). A call to heal: Entry patterns in Brazilian
mediumship. In C. W. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental
health: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 186-206). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Krippner, S. (in press). Cross-cultural treatment perspectives of
dissociative disorders. In S. J. Lynn
(Ed.), Dissociation: Clinical,
theoretical and research perspectives. NY. Guilford.
La Fontaine, J. (1990). Child sexual abuse. Cambridge, England: Polity Press.
Lambek, M. (1980). Spirits and spouses: Possession as a system
of communication among the Malagasy speakers of Mayotte. American
Ethnologist, 7, 318~331.
Lambek, M. (1988). Spirit possession/spirit succession: Aspects
of social continuity among Malagasy speakers in Mayotte. American
Ethnologist, 15, 7 10-7 3 1.
Lambek, M. (1989). From disease to discourse: Remarks on the
conceptualisation of trance and spirit possession. In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental health: Theoretical and
methodological issues (pp. 36-62).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lanning, K. V (1 989). Satanic, occult and ritualistic crime: A law
enforcement perspective. Police Chief 56, 62-85.
Larner, C. (1981). Enemies of God.. The witch-hunt
in Scotland. Baltimore: Johns
Hopkins University Press.
Leacock, S., & Leacock, R. (1972). Spirits
of the deep: A study of an Afro-Brazilian cult.
New York: Anchor Books.
Lee, R. L. M. (1989). Self-presentation in Malaysian spirit
seances: A dramaturgical perspective on altered states of consciousness in
healing ceremonies. In C. W. Ward (Ed.),
Altered states of consciousness and
mental health: Theoretical and methodological perspectives (pp. 251-266). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Lewis, I. M. (1987). Ecstatic religion. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness
testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Loftus, E. F. (1993a). Desperately seeking memories of the first few
years of childhood: The reality of early memories. Journal
of Experimental Psychology.. General, 122, 274-277.
Loftus, E. F. (1993b). The reality of repressed memories. American
Psychologist, 48, 518-537.
Loftus, E. F., Polonsky, S., & Fullilove,
M. T. (in press). Memories of childhood
sexual abuse: Remembering and repressing.
Psychology of Women Quarterly.
Lyons, A. (1988). Satan wants you: The cult of
devil worship in America. New York: Mysterious Press.
Maloney, H. N., & Lovekin, A. A. (1985).
Glossolalia: Behavioural science perspectives on speaking in
tongues. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Mancuso, J. C., & Sarbin, T. R.
(1983). The self narrative in the
enactment of roles. In T. R. Sarbin
& K. E. Schiebe (Eds.), Studies in social
identity (pp. 233-253). New York:
Praeger.
Merskey, H. (1992). The manufacture of personalities: The
production of multiple personality disorder.
British Journal of Psychiatry,
160. 327-340.
Merskey, H., & Buhrich, N. A.
(1975). Hysteria and organic brain
disease.
British Journal of Medical Psychology, 48, 359-366.
Michaelis, S. (1613). The admirable history of the
possession and conversion of a penitent woman.
London.
Miller, S. D., & Triggiano, P. J.
(1992). The psychophysiological investigation
of multiple personality disorder: Review and update. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 35, 47-6 1.
Modestin, J. (1992). Multiple personality disorder in Switzerland.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149,
88-92.
Moore, M.S. (1984). Law and psychiatry: Rethinking the relationship. Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
Moore, R. L. (1977). In
search of white crows. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Morton, A. (1977). Dawit: Competition and integration in an
Ethiopian Wuqabi cult group. In V
Crapanzano & V Garrison (Eds.), Case
studies in spirit possession (pp. 193-234).
New York: Wiley.
Mulhern, S. (1991a). Letter to the editor. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 15, 609-611.
Mulhern, S. (1991b). Satanism and psychotherapy: A rumour in search
of an inquisition. In J. T Richardson,
J. Rest, & D. G. Bromley (Eds.), The
Satanism scare (pp. 145-172).
Chicago: Aldine.
Mulhern, S. (in press). Le
trouble de la personnalite multiple a la recherche du trauma perdu [The
trouble with research on multiple personality and childhood trauma]. In M. Gabel, S. Lebovici, & P. Mazet
(Eds.), Inceste. Paris: Press Universitaire de France.
Nadon, R., Hoyt, 1. P., Register, P. A.,
& Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991).
Absorption and hypnotizability: Context effects reexamined. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 144-153.
Nash, M. R. (1 987). What, if anything, is regressed about
hypnotic age regression? A review of the
empirical literature. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 42-52.
Nash, M. R., Hulsey, T. L., Sexton, M. C.,
Harralson, T L., & Lambert, W. (1993).
Long-term sequelae of childhood sexual abuse: Perceived family
environment, psychopathology, and dissociation.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 61, 276-283.
Nash, M. R., & Lynn, S. J. (1986). Child abuse and hypnotic ability. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 5, 211-218.
Nash, M. R., Lynn, S. J., & Givens, D.
L. (1984). Adult hypnotic
susceptibility, childhood punishment, and child abuse: A brief
communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 32, 6-11.
Notestein, W. (1911). A
history of witchcraft in England. New
York: Crowell.
Oesterreich, T K. (1966). Possession: Demoniacal and
other. Secaucus, NJ: Citadel Press.
Ofshe, R. J. (1992). Inadvertent hypnosis during interrogation:
False confession due to dissociative state; mis-identified multiple personality
and the satanic cult hypothesis. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 40,125-156.
Ong, A. (1988). The production of possession: Spirits and the
multinational corporation in Malaysia. American Ethnologist, 15, 28-42.
Orne, M. T. (1979). On the simulating subject as a quasi-control
group in hypnosis research: What, why and how.
In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.),
Hypnosis: Developments in research and new perspectives (pp. 519-566). Chicago: Aldine.
Parfitt, D. N., & Gall, C. M. C.
(1944). Psychogenic amnesia: The refusal
to remember. Journal of Mental Science, 90, 511-531.
Perry, N. E. (1992). Therapists'
experiences of the effects of working with dissociative patients. Paper presented at the Ninth
International Conference on Multiple Personality/Dissociation, Arlington, VA.
Peters, L. G. (1981). An experiential study of Nepalese shamanism. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 13, 1-26.
Phoon, W. H. (1982). Outbreaks of mass hysteria at workplaces in
Singapore: Some patterns and modes of presentation. In M. J. Collinger, J. W. Pennebaker, &
L. R. Murray (Eds.), Mass psychogenic
illness: A social psychological analysis (pp. 21-52). Hiilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pope, H. G., & Hudson, J. 1.
(1992). Is childhood sexual abuse a risk
factor for bulimia nervosa? American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 455-463.
Prince, M. (1908). The
dissociation of a personality (2nd ed.). London: Longmans Green.
Putnam, F. W. (1989). Diagnosis and treatment of
multiple personality disorder. New
York: Guilford Press.
Putnam, F. W. (1993). Dissociative phenomena. In D. Spiegel (Ed.), Dissociative disorders: A clinical review (pp. 1-16).
Lutherville, MD: Sidran.
Putnam, F. W., Guroff, J. J., Silberman, E.
K., Barban, L., & Post, R. M. (1986).
The clinical phenomenology of multiple personality disorder: A review of
100 recent cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 47. 285-293.
Rabinowitz, F. E. (1989). Creating the multiple personality: An
experimental demonstration for an undergraduate abnormal psychology class.
Teaching of Psychology, 16, 69-71.
Radtke, H. L., & Spanos, N. P.
(1981). Was I hypnotised? A social psychological analysis of hypnotic
depth reports. Psychiatry, 44, 359-367.
Richardson, J. T. (1973). Psychological interpretations of glossolalia:
A re-examination of research. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 12, 199-207.
The Roman ritual of exorcism. (1976). In M. Malachi, Hostage to the devil. New
York: Reader's Digest Press. (Original work published 1614)
Rosenbaum, M. (1980). The role of the term schizophrenia in the
decline of the diagnosis of multiple personality. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 1383-1385.
Ross, C. A. (1989). Multiple personality
disorder.. Diagnosis, clinical features and treatment. New York: Wiley.
Ross, C. A. (1991). Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder
and dissociation. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 503-517.
Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., Fleisher, W P.,
& Norton, G. R. (1991). The
frequency of multiple personality disorder among psychiatric inpatients.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 148,
1717-1720.
Ross, C. A., Miller, S. D., Bjornson, L.,
Reagor, P., Fraser, G. A., & Anderson, G. (1991). Abuse histories in 102 cases of multiple
personality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 97-101.
Ross, C. A., Norton, C. R., & Fraser, G.
A. (1989). Evidence against the
iatrogenesis of multiple personality disorder.
Dissociation, 2, 61-65.
Ross, C. A., Norton, R., & Wozney, K.
(1989). Multiple personality disorder:
An analysis of 236 cases. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34,
414-418.
Russell, J. B. (1980). A
history of witchcraft. London:
Thames & Hudson.
Sarnarin, W. J. (1972). Tongues of men and angels: The
religious language of Pentecostalism. New
York: Macmillan.
Sansonnet-Hayden, H., Haley, G., Marriage,
K., & Fine, S. (1987). Sexual abuse
and psychopathology in hospitalised adolescents. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 753-757.
Sarbin, T. R. (in press). Dissociation: State, trait or skill? Contemporary
Hypnosis.
Sarbin, T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972). Hypnosis:
A social-psychological analysis of influence communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Saunders, L. W. (1977). Variants in Zar experience in an Egyptian
village. In V Crapanzano & V
Garrison (Eds.), Case studies in spirit
possession (pp. 177-19 1). New York:
Wiley.
Schieffelin, E. L. (1985). Performance and the cultural construction of
reality. American Ethnologist, 12, 707-724.
Schreiber, F. R. (1973). Sybil. New York: Warner.
Schwarz, T. (1981). The hillside strangler.. A
murderer's mind. New York: New
American Library.
Sharp, L. A. (1990). Possessed and dispossessed youth: Spirit
possession of school children in north-west Madagascar. Culture
Medicine and Psychiatry, 14, 339-364.
Shorter, E. (1992). From paralysis
to fatigue.. A history of psychosomatic medicine in the modern era. New York: Free Press.
Silva, C. E., & Kirsch, I. (1987). Breaching hypnotic amnesia by manipulating
expectancy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 325-329.
Smith, M. C. (1983). Hypnotic memory enhancement of witnesses:
Does it work? Psychological Bulletin, 94, 387-407.
Smith, M., & Pazder, L. (1980).
Michelle remembers. New York:
Pocket Books.
Spanos, N. P. (1978). Witchcraft in histories of psychiatry: A
critical analysis and an alternative conceptualisation. Psychological
Bulletin, 85, 417-439.
Spanos, N. P. (1983a). Demonic possession: A social psychological
analysis. In M. Rosenbaum (Ed.), Compliance behavior (pp. 149-198). New York: Free Press.
Spanos, N. P. (1983b). Ergotism and the Salem witch panic: A
critical analysis and an alternative conceptualisation. Journal
of the History of the behavioural Sciences, 19, 358-368.
Spanos, N. P. (1986a). Hypnosis and the modification of hypnotic
susceptibility: A social psychological perspective. In P. L. N. Naish (Ed.), What is hypnosis? (pp. 85-120). Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Spanos, N. P. (1986b). Hypnotic behavior- A social-psychological
interpretation of amnesia, analgesia and "trance logic." Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 9, 449-502.
Spanos, N. P. (1989). Hypnosis, demonic possession and multiple
personality: Strategic enactments and disavowals of responsibility for
actions. In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental
health: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 96-124). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Spanos, N. P., Arango, M., & de Groot,
H. P. (1993). Context as a moderator in
relationships between attribute variables and hypnotizability Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
19, 71-77.
Spanos, N. P., & Bodorik, H. L.
(1977). Suggested amnesia and
disorganised recall in hypnotic and task-motivated subjects. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 295-305.
Spanos, N. P., & Bures, E. (1993).
Pseudomemory creation and reversal in hypnotic, imagination control and
simulating subjects. Unpublished
manuscript, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada.
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., &
Burgess, M. F. (in press). Past life
identities, UFO abductions and satanic ritual abuse: The social construction of
"memories." International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.
Spanos, N. P., & Chaves, J. F. (1989).
Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioural perspective. Buffalo, NY.. Prometheus.
Spanos, N. P., & Coe, W. C. (1992). A social psychological approach to
hypnosis. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash
(Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp.
102-130). New York: Guilford Press.
Spanos, N. P., Cross, W. P., Lepage, M.,
& Coristine, M. (1986). Glossolalia
as learned behavior. An experimental
demonstration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 21-23.
Spanos, N. P., deGroot, H. P., Tiller, D.
K., Weekes, J. R., & Bertrand, L. D. (1985). "Trance logic" duality and hidden
observer responding in hypnotic, imagination control and simulating subjects. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 611-623.
Spanos, N. P., Flynn, D. M., & Gabora,
N. J. (1989). Suggested negative visual
hallucinations in hypnotic subjects: When no means yes. British
Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 6, 63-67.
Spanos, N. P., Flynn, D. M., & Gwynn, M.
I. (1988). Contextual demands, negative
hallucinations and hidden observer responding: Three hidden observers
observed. British Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 5, 5-10.
Spanos, N. P., Gabora, N. J., Jarrett, L.
E., & Gwynn, M. I. (1989). Contextual determinants of hypnotizability and
of the relationship between hypnotizability scales. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology 57, 271-278.
Spanos, N. P. & Gottlieb, J.
(1976). Ergotism and the Salem village
witch trials. Science, 194, 1390-1394.
Spanos, N. P., Gwynn, M. I., Corner, S. L.,
Baltruweit, W. J., & de Groh, M. (1989).
Are hypnotically induced pseudomemories resistant to
cross-examination? Law and Human Behavior, 13, 271-289.
Spanos, N. P., Gwynn, M. I., & Stam, H.
J. (1983). Instructional demands and
ratings of overt and hidden pain during hypnotic analgesia. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 92, 479-488.
Spanos, N. P., & Hewitt, E. C. (1
979). Glossolalia: A test of the
"trance" and psychopathology hypotheses. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 427-434.
Spanos, N. P., & Hewitt, E. C.
(1980). The hidden observer in hypnotic
analgesia: Discovery or experimental creation?
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 39, 1201-1214.
Spanos, N. P., Lush, N. I., & Gwynn, M.
I. (1989). Cognitive skill training
enhancement of hypnotizability: Generalisation effects and trance logic
responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,795-804.
Spanos, N. P., & Mclain, J. M.
(1986). Hypnotically created
pseudomemories: Memory distortions or reporting biases? British
Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis. 3, 155-159.
Spanos, N. P., Menary, E., Gabora, N. J.,
DuBreuil, S. C., & Dewhirst, B. (1991).
Secondary identity enactments during hypnotic past-life regression: A
socio-cognitive perspective. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 61, 308-320.
Spanos, N. P., Quigley, C. A., Gwynn, M. I.,
Glatt, R. L., & Perlini, A. H. (1991).
Hypnotic interrogation, pre-trial preparation, and witness testimony
during direct and cross-examination. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 639-653.
Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., &
Bertrand, L. D. (1984). Hypnotic amnesia
as a strategic enactment: Breaching amnesia in highly hypnotisable
subjects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1155-1169.
Spanos, N. R, Rivers, S. M., & Ross, S.
(1977). Experienced involuntariness and
response to hypnotic suggestions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
296, 208-221.
Spanos, N. P., Weekes, J. R., &
Bertrand, L. D. (1985). Multiple
personality: A social psychological perspective. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 94, 362-376.
Spanos, N. P., Weekes, J. R., Menary, E.,
& Bertrand, L. D. (1986). Hypnotic interview and age regression procedures
in the elicitation of multiple personality symptoms: A simulation study. Psychiatry,
49, 298-311.
Stava, L. J., & Jaffa, M. (1988). Some operationalizations of the
neodissociation concept and their relationship to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 989-996.
Stoller, P. (1989). Stressing social change in Songhay
possession. In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental
health: A cross cultural perspective (pp. 267-284). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Sutcliffe, J. P., & Jones, J.
(1962). Personal identity, multiple
personality and hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 10, 231-269.
Takahashi, Y. (1990). Is multiple personality disorder really rare
in Japan?
Dissociation, 3, 5 7-59.
Talamini, J. T (1982). Boys will be girls: The hidden
world of the heterosexual male transvestite.
Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
Teoh, J., Soewondo, S., & Sidharta, M.
(1975). Epidemic hysteria in Malaysian
schools: An illustrative example. Psychiatry, 38, 258-268.
Terr, L. (1988). What happens to early memories of
trauma? A study of twenty children under
age five at the time of documented traumatic events. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 96-104.
Terr, L. (1990). Too scared to cry. New York: Harper & Row.
Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. M.
(1957). The three faces of Eve. New
York: Fawcett.
Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. M.
(1984). On the incidence of multiple
personality disorder. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 32, 63-66.
Thomas, K. (1971). Religion
and the decline of magic. New York:
Scribner.
Venkataramaiah, V, Mallikarjunaiah, M.,
Chandra, C. R., Rao, C. K., & Reddy, G. N. (1981). Possession syndrome: An epidemiological study
in West Karnataka. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 23, 213-218.
Victor, J. S. (1993). Satanic panic: The creation of
a contemporary legend. Peru, IL:
Open Court.
Wagstaff, G. E (1981). Hypnosis, compliance and
belief New York: St. Martin's Press.
Wagstaff, G. F. (1989). Forensic aspects of hypnosis. In N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioural
perspective (pp. 340-357). Buffalo,
NY. Prometheus.
Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R.
(1991). Female child sexual abusers: A
critical review of the literature. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 9, 43-69.
Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R.
(1992). Recovered memories of alleged
sexual abuse: Lawsuits against parents. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 10, 483-507.
Walker, D. P. (1981). Unclean spirits. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Press.
Wambaugh, H. (1979). Life
before life. New York: Bantam.
Ward, C. A. (1989). Possession and exorcism: Psychopathology and
psychotherapy in a magico-religious context.
In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states
of consciousness and mental health: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp.
125-144). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Watkins, J. G. (1 984). The Bianchi (L.A. hillside strangler) case:
Sociopath or multiple personality. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 32, 67- 1 0 1.
Wells, G. L., Ferguson, T. J., &
Lindsay, R. C. L. (1 98 1). The
tractability of eyewitness confidence and its implications for triers of
fact. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 688-696.
Whitley, G. (1992, January). Abuse of trust. D
Magazine, pp. 36-39.
Wijesinghe, C. P., Dissanayake, S. A. W.,
& Mendis, N. (1976). Possession
trance in a semi-urban community in Sri Lanka.
Australian and New Zealand Journal
of Psychiatry, 11, 93-100.
Wilbur, C.B.(1984).Treatment of multiple
personality. Psychiatric Annals, 14, 27-31.
Young, W. C., Sachs, R. G., Braun, B. G.,
& Watkins, R. T. (1991). Patients reporting ritual abuse in childhood: A
clinical syndrome. Report of 37
cases. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 181~189.