The Journal of Mind and Behaviour Winter
and Spring 1991, Volume 15, Numbers 1 and 2 Pages
139-156
Neuroleptic Drug Treatment of Schizophrenia: The
State of the Confusion
David Cohen
Universite de
Montreal
Abstract
This article contends that
the enterprise of neuroleptic drug treatment of
schizophrenia is conceptually and clinically -though not economically
-bankrupt. Although new drugs spur hope and reinforce the dominant treatment
paradigm, evidence from reports published during the last five years in leading
psychiatric journals suggests that psychopharmacologists
do not know what are the optimal doses of the most widely- used neuroleptics; that most patients do not "respond"
to neuroleptic treatment; that toxic effects are
routinely misdiagnosed; that prescribing guidelines may have no impact on
actual prescription patterns; that claims that the popular "atypical"
neuroleptic clozapine is
free of extrapyramidal symptoms are completely false;
and finally, that penetration of the double-blind in studies of the
effectiveness of psychotropics over placebos may be a
common occurrence. In the light of these findings, it is argued that the field
is in crisis and that major, paradigmatic change is absolutely necessary.
Introduction
The major change this
century in the psychiatric treatment of the various conditions labelled "schizophrenia" or "the
schizophrenias" has come from the introduction of chlorpromazine and other
phenothiazines in the early 1950s. This was followed
by similar drugs variously called major tranquilizers, antipsychotics or neuroleptics. For nearly 40 years, apart from the studies
and reviews of a very small group of researchers, including psychiatric critics
(e.g., Breggin, 1983, 1991; Ciompi,
Dauwalder, Maier, Aebi, Trtitsch et al., 1992; Cohen, 1988; Fischer and Greenberg,
1989; Karon, 1989; Kiesler and Sibulkin,
1987; Mosher and Menn, 1978; Paul and Lentz, 1977;
Warner, 1985), there has been near-universal consensus in the scientific
literature, as well as in popular media reports, that neuroleptics
have been the most useful treatment for schizophrenic psychoses, unsurpassed by
any other form of intervention. This attitude is well reflected in the opening
sentences of a recent research report, which reads: "The antipsychotic
efficacy of neuroleptics has been confirmed in
numerous studies based on a meticulous method . . . It is only antipsychotic
medication that enables many patients to benefit from [other
interventions]" (Windgassen, 1992, p. 405,
references deleted).
This consensus showed, from
the early 1980s onward, some signs of strain. The behavioral toxicity and
numerous iatrogenic effects of neuroleptics
{especially the late-appearing involuntary movement disorders) came to be
increasingly recognized, or, more to the point, increasingly discussed by leading
psychiatric researchers and the American Psychiatric Association {e.g., APA,
1985, 1992; Task Force, 1980; Van Putten and Marder, 1987). In 1986, even Pierre Deniker (credited, with
Jean Delay, of having introduced chlorpromazine in psychiatry), published an
article entitled "Are the Antipsychotic Drugs to be Withdrawn?" (Deniker
answered his question in the negative.)
However, these minor doubts
appear to have given way, in the early 1990s, to a wave of renewed optimism {at
least in North America) about the drug treatment of schizophrenia, one based
partly on the introduction of new or formerly shelved antipsychotics such as risperidone and clozapine. These
compounds, loosely referred to as "atypical" neuroleptics
because their dopamine receptor binding differs from that of most drugs
currently in use, are stated to be equal to or superior than the older neuroleptics, especially for
"treatment-resistant" or "neuroleptic
non-responsive" patients, and to pro- duce vastly fewer side effects. For
example, the advertisement for risperidone in the
April 1994 issue of the American Journal of Psychiatry states that "incidence
and severity of extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS) were
similar to placebo." (1) These newer drugs create, in the minds of many
users, such as patients, families, governmental bodies, and the media, the
impression that although there continue to be many hurdles to understanding and
treating schizophrenia, there is nevertheless progress. This in turn reinforces
the dominant paradigm in North America that "schizophrenia"
represents some sort of genetically predisposed, environmentally triggered, neurodevelopmental brain disease which, at this state of
our knowledge, best responds to chemical intervention. (2)
Nevertheless, the position
advanced in this paper is that the neuroleptic drug
treatment of schizophrenia is today, quite simply, in a mess. This thesis will
be supported by a selective review of research reports published during the
last five years in leading psychiatric and medical journals, reports touching
on basic aspects of use, prescription, and evaluation of neuroleptic
and other prescribed psychiatric drugs. This review leads to the suggestion
that the enterprise of drug treatment of schizophrenia is conceptually and
clinically - though certainly not economically - bankrupt, and calls for major
paradigmatic change. That such change is urged only by critics of psychiatry or
by researchers in other disciplines reinforces Kuhn's (1970) oft-cited thesis
about the powerful inertia of scientific systems; or Karon's
(1989, p. 146) conclusion (following an in-depth review of medication versus
psychotherapy studies) that "political and economic factors and a
concentration on short- term cost-effectiveness, rather than the scientific
findings, currently seem to dictate [drug treatment of schizophrenia)"; or
Cohen and McCubbin's (1990) observation that
systematic power imbalances between interested parties in the drug prescription
situation ensure that only "scientific findings" favouring
the interests of the most powerful parties will be legitimated as such.
This paper discusses the
results and implications of a small number of published reports of
investigations about neuroleptics and other psychotropics conducted by different researchers. No
attempt is made to uncover and review all the literature bearing on the topic,
and no claim is made that the findings from these studies are representative of
findings of most studies on drug treatment. However, it will become clear that
evidence to corroborate the main thesis is widely available. In all
probability, such evidence will continue to accumulate, along with recognition
that the leading treatment approach is not only inadequate, but is the source
of the problems here discussed.
The
State of the Confusion
What
Dose Should We Use?
The basic factor involved in
the prescription of any drug for the treatment of any undesirable
condition is determining appropriate dose. After nearly 40 years of intensive
psychopharmacological research and clinical experience with neuroleptics
on hundreds of millions of patients throughout the world, one would expect the
dosage of neuroleptic drug administration to be well-mapped.
In particular, considering that neuroleptic use is
associated with serious toxic effects, many of which appear to be
dose-dependent, one would also expect the minimum effective dosage of various neuroleptic agents to be known. This, however, is not
exactly the case. The phrase "minimum effective dosage" is important,
since it is specifically what the official APA prescribing guidelines urge
clinicians to utilize in chronic treatment (APA, 1992, p. 251).
In a study by Rifkin, Doddi, Karajgi, Borenstein, and Wachspress
(1991), 87 newly admitted inpatients with a diagnosis
of schizophrenia were randomly assigned to three neuroleptic-treatment
conditions: one group received 10 mg/day of haloperidol, one group received 30
mg/day, and a third group received 80 mg/day. Subjects were then evaluated
(using state-of-the-art symptom scales) under double-blind conditions for six
weeks. At the end of this period, no differences in clinical condition were
noted between the three groups.
To understand the
significance of this finding, one must remember that haloperidol has been in
use for about 30 years and is considered the "gold standard"
treatment for schizophrenia ("Major advance," 1993). It was estimated
to be the second most-frequently prescribed neuroleptic
in the United States in 1985 (Wysowsky and Baum,
1989). Its effects on animals and humans are extremely well documented.
Furthermore, the difference between 10 mg and 80 mg of haloperidol is very
great, roughly equivalent to the difference between 500 mg and 4000 mg of
chlorpromazine (at the time of the study, 20-25 mg/day of haloperidol was
considered a "standard dose"). Still, what these results suggest is
that, after all this time and clinical experience with haloperidol, we still
cannot predict different effects of gross dosage variations of this drug, nor
do we know what is its optimal dose (for the treatment of acute
"schizophrenia").
Is this ignorance limited to
haloperidol, or are we in the dark about other drugs? One answer is given by
Kane (1989), who notes that it is still not possible to relate blood level of a
neuroleptic to observed clinical response and that
"questions remain as to what specific drug should be used [and] what
dosage for what duration is needed. . ." (p. 323). Another, more
diplomatic answer is found in a report by Waddington, Weller, Crow, and Hirsch
(1992), summarizing the presentations at a recent international conference on
schizophrenia: ". . .there is renewed
appreciation of our previous failure to establish, even at this late stage in
their evolution, the optimal usage of existing typical neuroleptic
drugs and of the potential benefit still to be gained therefrom"
(p. 994, emphasis added). Bitter, Volavka, and Scheurer ( 1991 ), for their part,
are more direct: "Despite intensive research and after almost four decades
of neuroleptic treatment we still do not know the
minimum effective dose of any neuroleptic" (p.
32).
What
is the Rate of Nonresponse to Neuroleptic
Treatment?
Since the recent
introduction (in North America) of clozapine, a drug
marketed specifically for "neuroleptic nonresponders," we have been hearing very much about
this group of schizophrenic patients. Previous discussions of this particular
difficulty with neuroleptic treatment were rare, and
one would get the impression from the literature that almost
no one did not "respond" to such treatment, especially since it is
administered to the overwhelming majority of people diagnosed as schizophrenic.
Recently, though, some reports estimated a 5-25% nonresponse
rate (e.g., Brenner, Dencker, Goldstein, Hubbard,
Keegan et al., 1990). Informed observers have suspected that the rate is much
higher (e.g., Easton and Link, 1986-87).
The most typical standard to
evaluate the effectiveness of neuroleptic treatment
of schizophrenics has been that of relapse (usual.ly defined as rehospitalization for an acute psychotic episode, or a
marked increase in symptoms as measured by instruments such as the Brief
Psychiatric Rating Scale). Generally, two groups of comparable patients, one
administered neuroleptics and the other a placebo,
are followed for a determined period (usually 4 to 12 months, occasionally 24
months) after release from an index hospitalization. Effectiveness is measured
by comparing the number of patients who relapse in each group. Hundreds of neuroleptic effectiveness studies have been carried out
since the late 1950s, and the overall rate of effectiveness reported is very
similar to the rate recently estimated by Davis, Kane, Marder,
Brauzer, Gierl et al. (1993)
from 35 random-assignment, double-blind studies involving 3720 patients:
"patients on placebo relapse at a rate of 55%, whereas only 21% of schizophrenic
patients relapse when they are on maintenance therapy" (p. 24).
Subtracting from the placebo rate the 21% of patients who would have relapsed
even if they were on drugs, we obtain the net effectiveness rate of 34%, or one
in three schizophrenic patients for whom neuroleptics
appear to delay relapse during a set study period.
To properly evaluate the
effectiveness of neuroleptics in schizophrenia,
however, one should consider, in addition to relapse, key outcome measures like
social integration and employment, that are now taken
into account in most long-term outcome studies. Yet,
according to Meltzer (1992), "there are no studies that demonstrate the
outcome of neuroleptic treatment in schizophrenia
using all these criteria" (p. 516, emphasis added).
The results of an unusual
investigation by Keck, Cohen, Baldessarini, and
McElroy (1989) raise other disturbing questions concerning the effectiveness of
neuroleptics. These authors reviewed relevant studies
in order to define the onset and time course of antipsychotic effects of neuroleptic drugs. They excluded open trials, studies of
chronically psychotic patients, and studies not using a placebo or non-neuroleptic sedative as a control, which left only five
reports out of more than 1,300 published studies on the efficacy of neuroleptics. In the three studies of neuroleptic
vs. placebo, and the two of neuroleptic vs. sedative,
"[T]he same overall degree of improvement was observed during treatment. . . within each of the markedly different time
intervals studied. Furthermore, when a neuroleptic
was compared to a sedative - diazepam or opium powder - the sedative
demonstrated efficacy similar to that of the neuroleptic
during the first day and through 4 weeks of treatment" (pp. 1290-1291,
references deleted). In a letter to the editor commenting on these results, an
admittedly baffled psychiatrist wondered: "Has our clinical judgment about
the efficacy of antipsychotics been a fixed, encapsulated, delusional perception. . .? If there is no difference
in outcome in a month, how about 2 months, or 6, or a year, or a lifetime?
Do sedatives prevent relapse as well as antipsychotics do? Are we back to square 1 in antipsychotic psychopharmacology?" (Turns, 1990, p. 1576).
We may now return to the
question posed at the start of this section: What is the rate of "nonresponse" to neuroleptic
treatment for acute episodes of schizophrenia? One answer is found in the
results of a study con- ducted by Johns, Mayerhoff,
Lieberman, and Kane (1990), which was published as a chapter in a book entitled
The Neuroleptic Nonresponsive Patient. In this study,
researchers first administered a standard dose of a high-potency neuroleptic (20 mg/day of fluphenazine)
to 29 "acutely exacerbated, hospitalized chronic schizophrenic
patients," and obtained a response rate of 37%. Although
this seemed "surprisingly low" to the authors, review of an earlier
pilot study undertaken with 31 similar patients "revealed an almost
identical response rate (35%) to the same treatment condition" (p. 62).
The authors found that "Only one-third of such patients responded well to
an initial 4- week course of neuroleptic treatment;
continued neuroleptic treatment for an additional 4
weeks regardless of whether the neuroleptic class or
dose was changed or held steady, resulted in almost no further improvement in
clinical condition" (p. 63). Additional data from this ongoing study has
been published, with the sample size increased to 156 "acutely ill
schizophrenic, schizoaffective, and schizophreniform"
hospitalized patients (Kinon, Kane, Johns, Perovich, Ismi et al., 1993). Of
the 115 patients who completed the first four-week phase of the study, 68% were
rated as non-responders. Of the nonresponders who
went on to randomized treatment (lower dose, higher Jose, or other neuroleptic), "only 4 of 47 subjects (9%) subsequently
responded" (p. 309). Despite their earlier surprise, the authors now
characterize the 68% nonresponse rate as
"consistent with a range in previous reports" (p.310).
Because of the scarcity of
systematic studies focusing on nonresponse, it is
difficult to assess how common this response is in typical practice. However,
Collins, Hogan, and Awad (1992) rated 50% of all schizophrenic patients
hospitalized for more than six months in Ontario's largest psychiatric hospital
as nonresponders {although these patients were
maintained on daily neuroleptic doses as high as
acute patients!). Further, Meltzer's {1992) comprehensive review of treatment
strategies for neuroleptic nonrespondents
begins by estimating, matter-of-factly, that up to 45% of patients do not
respond to neuroleptics or develop such severe
drug-induced behavioral toxi- city that treatment
cannot be continued.
Can
We Tell a Side Effect When We See One?
The issue of side effects
(3) resulting from neuroleptic treatment is certainly
the most daunting problem in this field today. It has
received an enormous amount of attention during the last few years, including
books with such titles as Adverse Effects of Psychotropic Drugs (Kane and
Lieberman, 1992), or the more evocative Drug-Induced Dysfunction in Psychiatry
(Keshavan and Kennedy, 1992), or Toxic Psychiatry (Breggin, 1991).
Most patients receiving neuroleptic drugs will experience one or another of a
multitude of undesirable effects, ranging from sedation, dry mouth, loss of
sexual desire, to various acute or tardive movement
disorders known as extrapyramidal symptoms {EPS);
these include parkinsonism, dystonia, akathisia, and tardive dyskinesia. According to the manufacturers of risperidone, EPS "are observed in 75 to 90% of
patients on neuroleptic therapy and are the major
cause of noncompliance and relapse" ("Effect of risperidone",
1993, p. 1). (4) In their tardive forms, EPS rarely
respond to any treatment and are usually irreversible {Gualtieri,
1993). The situation is further complicated by the fact that some of the most
common manifestations of acute EPS, such as akinetic
depression or akathisic agitation, are
indistinguishable from, respectively, psychotic withdrawal or agitation,
considered to be core symp- toms of schizophrenia
{Rifkin, 1987; Van Putten and Marder,
1987).
It is thus important to know
how clinicians detect the presence of neuroleptic-induced
behavioral toxicity. To this author's knowledge, the only published study to
have specifically addressed this issue was conducted by Weiden,
Mann, Haas, Mattson, and Frances (1987). The investigators compared
well-trained clinicians' recognition of the major EPS in 48 psychotic inpatients with independent blind diagnoses by researchers
using standardized rating scales. For all types of EPS, there were striking
rates of disagreement between the research and the clinical diagnoses. Only one
of ten patients with tardive dyskinesia,
seven of 27 patients with akathisia, and 17 of 26
patients with parkinsonism were accurately diagnosed
by the clinicians. Furthermore, every single case of acute EPS that was
recognized by clinicians was initially treated by adding another drug, an antiparkinsonian. Not once did clinicians reduce the neuroleptic dose, which had caused the EPS in the first
place. Without significant changes in diagnostic and medical training, the
authors conclude, "it is likely that extrapyramidal
side effects will continue to be underdiagnosed at an
alarmingly high rate" (p. 1153). In a later study, it was suggested that a
four-hour course in diagnosis and management of EPS resulted in better
recognition of EPS and lower neuroleptic doses prescribed
by psychiatric residents (Dixon, Weiden, Frances, and
Rapkin, 1989). Reviewing the curricula of five local
psychiatric residency programs, the investigators found that "a mean of
only 0.5 hours" (p. 104) is spent specifically on EPS.
Are
We Getting Better at Prescribing Neuroleptics?
In a rare follow-up study of
prescription patterns conducted on a sizeable group (N= 253) of chronic
psychiatric patients; Segal, Cohen, and Marder (1992)
compared psychotropic drug prescriptions in the sample in 1973 and 1985. The
initial impetus of the study was to document how, in light of published
reports, professional guidelines, adverse publicity about the dangers of EPS
and an increasing amount of tardive dyskinesia (TD)-related litigation, psychiatrists had
(probably) modified their prescribing habits, in conformity with the 1980s
consensus about neuroleptic use. This consensus
included, among other recommendations, lowering doses for patients on long-term
treatment. The findings: over the 12-year period, daily doses doubled, from
about 500 mg/day in chlorpromazine-equivalent in 1973, to about 1000 mg/day in
1985 (almost identical results are reported by Reardon, Rifkin, Schwartz,
Myerson, and Siris (1989), in a multi-center
longitudinal study, from 1973 to 1982).
More unexpectedly, patients
prescribed their drugs by psychiatrists, versus those prescribed drugs by non
psychiatric physicians, were receiving the highest doses at follow-up (even
when other variables -such as number of hospitalizations, psychiatric symptoms,
place of residence, etc. -were held constant). Thus, non psychiatric physicians, less trained in the use of neuroleptics
and less sensitized to their adverse effects, were more prudent in their
prescribing than psychiatrists. In addition to using lower doses, nonpsychiatrists prescribed less potent neuroleptics
and fewer concomitant drugs.
There are numerous
indications in the recent literature that "low" doses of neuroleptics, in the range of 3-5mg/day of oral haloperidol
(the so-called threshold dose5) may be effective for about 70% of acutely
psychotic schizophrenic patients within five weeks (e.g. Hogarty,
1993). It remains to be seen how - and when - such findings will be applied in
everyday clinical settings. There is, however, no reason to be optimistic.
Segal et al. (1992) suggest that prescribers respond to varied imperatives when
deciding how to prescribe drugs. Lacking tools other than psychotropic drugs to
prevent schizophrenic relapses, they are reticent to err on the side of caution
by prescribing less. Cohen and McCubbin (1990)
suggest that information on drugs or side effects cannot, by itself, lead to a
change in psychiatric prescription patterns -the field is
simply not governed by scientific considerations, nor are the ultimate
consumers, the patients, in any position to effect change.
Are
We Responsible Enough to Use Neuroleptics?
Clozapine is said to be an
"atypical" antipsychotic because it appears to bind selectively to different dopamine receptors than the vast
majority of neuroleptics routinely available in North
America. This substance, used modestly in Europe since the early 1960s, had its
use greatly restricted after a series of about 20 deaths due to agranulocytosis (sharp drop in white blood cells) in 1975
in Finland and Switzerland (Kerwin, 1994). The other
major drawback of clozapine is a tendency to cause
convulsions, in a dose-related manner. In 1990, Sandoz reintroduced clozapine in Canada and the United States, with great media
fanfare (including a Time magazine cover story published on July 6, 1992), as a
treatment effective for the (suddenly) numerous category of nonresponsive and
treatment-resistant patients (estimated at 30% in the Time article). Healy (1993)
notes that "With the problems of launching clozapine
in the US and the UK owing to its toxicity, company- sponsored research has
focused on a treatment-resistance indication," although previous studies
from Europe showed that the drug's efficacy for schizophrenia "has been no
more and no less than that of other neuroleptic
agents" (p. 25 ).
As an added, incredible
bonus, clozapine was depicted in practically every publication, and every advertisement by Sandoz, to be
remarkably free of EPS.6 For example, the advertisement for clozapine
in the January 1990 issue of the American Journal of Psychiatry contains the
following headline: "Hope continues with a virtual absence of certain
acute extrapyramidal symptoms." Some
researchers, such as Schwartz and Brotman ( 1992), state simply that clozapine
"does not cause extrapyramidal effects" (p.
981 ). This affirmation was, in almost every instance, accompanied by a
statement to the effect that there had not yet been any "confirmed
cases" of tardive dyskinesia
associated with clozapine.
A mere three years later,
aside from several reports of clozapine-induced side
effects typical of phenothiazine neuroleptics,
such as major weight gain (Wiebe, 1993 ), priapism (Rosen and Hano, 1992; Seftel, Saenz de Tejada, Szetela, Cole, and Goldstein, 1992; Ziegler and Behar,
1992), and anticholinergic delirium or toxic
psychosis (Szymanski, Jody, Leipzig, Masiar, and
Lieberman, 1991), there is at least one published report of clozapine-associated
tardive dyskinesia (DeLeon, Moral, and Camufias,
1991; an earlier report of tardive dyskinesia exacerbation with clozapine
was reported in the German literature by Doepp and Buddeberg, 1975). For their part, Nobecourt
and T urgeon ( 1992) state
that, in the few years since clozapine's
introduction, "a few cases of mild tardive dyskinesia have been reported" (p. 71 ).
There are also several
reports of the occurrence of a typical symptom of parkinsonism,
hypersalivation (Bourgeois, Drexler, and Hall, 1991;
Grabowski, 1992). There are also several reports of akathisia
(e.g., Friedman, 1993, including one blind survey in which akathisia
was observed to be similar in prevalence and severity in patients treated with clozapine and those treated with standard neuroleptics, with a worse overall clinical outcome for akathisic patients regardless of the neuroleptic
used (Cohen, Keck, Satlin, and Cole, 1991). There are
also reports such as the following summary of the results of a two-year
prospective clozapine monitoring program carried out
by W. W. Fleischhaker: "Surprisingly, some 10%
of patients developed mild to moderate akathisia, and
almost 40% developed tremor" (Waddington et al., 1992, p. 993). Finally,
several reports have appeared about a rare but quintessential neuroleptic effect, the potentially fatal neuroleptic malignant syndrome (e.g., DasGupta
and Young, 1991; Miller, Sharafuddin, and Kathol, 1991; Nemecek, Rastogi-Cruz, and Csernansky,
1993; Reddig, Minnema, and Tanjon, 1993), although the diagnosis in some of these cases
has been questioned (Weller and Korhuber, 1993).
Simply put, clozapine is not so "atypical." Still, as late as
July 1993, in no less a prestigious journal than The New England Journal of
Medicine, one could read, "Unlike classic neuroleptic
agents, clozapine is not associated with the
development of acute extrapyramidal symptoms or tardive dyskinesia" (Alvir, Lieberman, Safferman, Schwimmer, and Schaaf, 1993, p.
162).
Error or deception?
In a 1989 retrospective,
Deniker, commenting on how his and Delay's definition of the characteristics of
neuroleptics (as agents necessarily having neurological
toxicity) had withstood the test of time, had this to say about clozapine:
In 1970, Stille
and Hippius announced that clozapine
was a powerful antipsychotic without extrapyramidal
effects: our theory was therefore seriously attacked. In reality, this was the
exception that proves the rule. We had already experimented with clozapine, and had abandoned it on account of neurovegetative phenomena (collapse) which in the range of
effects of neuroleptics are symmetrical to the extrapyramidal symptoms; but these are certainly
neurological manifestations. (p. 256)
Recently, Healy (1993)
posited several reasons why the efficacy of clozapine
may have been vastly over-estimated, why in fact a satisfactory double-blind
study of maintenance treatment with clozapine may
have been impossible to conduct. Among others, he mentions the fact that the
"combination of excitement and close supervision [weekly or twice-weekly
blood counts] of results can be expected to be associated with better response
rates than the current neglect that is all too often visited on chronic schizophrenic
patients" (p. 26). But this may be a moot point. There is no shortage of
alternative antipsychotics, currently in different phases of clinical testing
or regulatory approval and estimated to cost a fraction of the cost of clozapine, to take its place (Hollister, 1994). This is, of
course, the case with risperidone, recently approved
for clinical use in North America. During a conversation with me, a
psychiatrist involved in clinical studies with the drug summed up his
enthusiasm in the following words: "This drug is so amazing, patients are
getting better faster than their illness allows." What seems amazing is
not the power of psychoactive substances but the expectant faith and naive
rhetoric of some clinicians. This pharmaceutically fueled faith, not any valid
new or improved understanding of the nature of schizophrenia, is undoubtedly
the driving force in the field today. This is illustrated by the title of an
editorial in the British Journal of Psychiatry: "The New Atypical
Antipsychotics: A lack of Extrapyramidal Side-Effects
and New Routes of Schizophrenia Research" (Kerwin,
1994). It is also supported by the following quote from Mitchell (1993):
Forty years after the
discovery of chlorpromazine finds us with the enthusiasm of the introduction of
clozapine. At the same time, however, it is sobering
to reflect on how little we have learned of the aetiology
of the functional psychoses, despite the fervor generated by the excitement of
the psychopharmacological discoveries of the 1950s. (p.344)
Chlorpromazine and clozapine might have more in common than being introduced
40 years apart and generating excitement in the psychiatric pro- fession. Other parallels come to mind: exaggerated
therapeutic claims; widely- publicized personal accounts of near-miraculous
recoveries; selective denial and misperception of obvious "side
effects"; and relief at not having to deal fully with the public health
consequences of the iatrogenic effects of previously acclaimed, equally
miraculous treatments. There exists a refusal to acknowledge, let alone
discuss, the immense theoretical and practical contradictions generated by the
abandonment of these previous treatments. Today, Kerwin
(1994) recommends that atypical antipsychotics "replace classic
antipsychotics" in routine clinical practice (p. 146). The cycle continues,
until another pharmaceutical innovation will lay bare the disadvantages of
today's novelty.
Clozapine and its
new-and-improved successors confront us with a basic question which researchers
in the 1950s and 1960s had answered in the negative. Can we obtain the
antipsychotic, agitation-reducing effect without producing an equivalently
profound toxic effect? Can we expect the human brain to absorb drug induced
disruptions in neurotransmission without compensating by symmetrical behavioral
toxicity? Whatever answer we give to this key question, it is useful to
remember that, to this day, selective suppression of avoidance behavior,
inhibition of spontaneous locomotor activity, and the
production of catalepsy in exposed laboratory rats - not merely some
biochemical measure of dopamine receptor binding - still serve as the best markers
of potential "antipsychotic" potency of compounds in the early phases
of clinical investigation (Ahlenius, 1991).
How
Do We Evaluate the Therapeutic Effectiveness of Drugs?
In clinical drug research in
which the efficacy of an experimental drug is compared with a placebo
substance, "blindability" is an extremely
important element. It usually implies that drug and placebo are coded and
dispensed in identical-appearing form so that neither the subject nor the
evaluator/clinician knows which treatment is being given to whom. This is done
in order to counteract the well-documented effects of subject or experimenter
expectation on the evaluation of the benefits of a particular drug. Blind-
ability takes on added importance, in the light of recent compelling reports
which estimate that up 70% of patients affected with mild medical conditions
respond very favorably to placebo (Goleman, 1993).
White, Kando,
Park, Waternaux, and Brown (1992) retrospectively
assessed the "blindability" of a clinical
drug trial of etoperidone, a putative antidepressant.
An evaluator was provided with all the drug trial data minus the outcome
results. The evaluator was then asked to guess which subjects had received the
experimental drug and which subjects had received the placebo. The evaluator
correctly guessed active drug assignment for 73% of the 22 etoperidone-treated
subjects, and 67% of the placebo-treated subjects, and this on the basis of
side effects alone. Distinguishing drugs from placebo on this basis was
suggested in an early review by Breggin (1983, p.
59). According to more recent, comprehensive reviews (Greenberg, Bornstein,
Greenberg, and Fischer, 1992; Fischer and Greenberg, 1993), side effects
(including the fact that inert placebos simply do not produce the variety and
intensity of physical sensations which active substances do) may be the primary
reason for penetration of the double-blind. In their meta-analysis of 22
studies of antidepressant effectiveness, Greenberg et al. found that if, in
addition to the new drug being tested, some patients were given an older antidepressant
as a control, the new drug was only one quarter to one half as powerful as
reported in studies in which the new drug was tested only against an inert
placebo. In any case, according to Fischer and Greenberg ( 1993 ), there now
exists "a substantial reservoir of data discrediting the integrity of the
double-blind ...[which] means that most past studies of the efficacy of
psychotropic drugs are to unknown degrees scientifically untrustworthy"
(pp. 345; 348). In other words, we may be justified to question seriously the
validity of the very large volume of "controlled" clinical
psychotropic drug research.
Conclusion
This paper has attempted to
support the contention that the neuroleptic drug
treatment of schizophrenia, contrary to numerous statements contained in
professional and popular reports, and despite the arrival of "new and
improved" antipsychotics, is at a virtual standstill and that no real
progress has been made since the introduction of these drugs forty years ago.
The argument is based on the following evidence.
First, psychopharmacologists
do not know what are the optimal or minimally effective doses
of the most widely-used neuroleptic drugs.
Second, the rate of nonresponse to neuroleptic
treatment in acute and chronic schizophrenic patients is probably in the 45-70%
range, not the previously stated 5-25% range. Furthermore, the net positive
effect of neuroleptics, with respect to relapse
prevention over a one- to two-year period, is visible only in one third of
patients.
Third, even well-trained
clinicians may routinely fail to recognize text- book presentations of acute
EPS. When these EPS are accurately diagnosed, subsequent medication decisions
may compound the problem.
Fourth, contrary to
recommendations from the research literature and from official guidelines,
psychiatrists who prescribed neuroleptics to chronic
patients until the mid-1980s had a tendency to increase doses over time.
Fifth, despite frequent,
unequivocal statements by renowned psychopharmacologists
in the most prestigious psychiatric and medical journals to the effect that clozapine, a novel antipsychotic, is "remarkably
free" of typical EPS, easily available evidence suggests that this is
simply a false claim.
Sixth, research protocols
used to determine whether a psychotropic drug is more effective than placebo
may be fundamentally flawed, since the appearance of side effects has been
shown to negate the blindability of clinical
investigations.
Some of the above statements
relate to basic knowledge about the effects of antipsychotic drugs, while
others are more closely tied to how the drugs are used and promoted in everyday
practice. One might argue that to mix both these aspects of neuroleptic
treatment may be misleading and fail to give a true picture of this form of
treatment. However, scientific studies are supposed to inform clinical
practice, which, in turn, may suggest lines for scientific inquiry. What is one
to make of the fact that undesirable, toxic effects of neuroleptics,
unarguably more frequent and predictable than therapeutic effects, are less
understood, studied, and known than therapeutic effects?
It may be also argued that,
although the drug treatment of schizophrenia still "remains a quagmire for
clinicians" ("Drug treatment," 1992), there are indications
today that clinicians are prescribing more prudently and may be returning to
the use of typical doses more reminiscent of the 1950s and 1960s. For example,
recent work on the neuroleptic threshold dose has
highlighted the advantages of low-dose treatment compared to standard or high
doses. All this may be well and true. By the same token, however, we have been
through this before. And it has brought us to where we are today. Lower dose
treatment and a frank admission that extrapyramidal
symptoms were a sine qua non of antipsychotic efficacy (e.g., Denber, 1959) did not really make a difference in the
rational use of neuroleptics: clinicians were
obviously not satisfied with the results obtained, and increased neuroleptic doses to high levels, with the results
described here which any experienced clinician would recognize.7 In conclusion,
it is reasonable to entertain the suggestion that in any other field of applied
scientific endeavour, results such as these would
indicate that the field is in crisis, that conventional assumptions are wrong,
and that major, paradigmatic change is absolutely necessary.
References
Ahlenius, S.
(1991). Pharmacological evaluarion
of new antipsychotic drugs. In P. Willner
(Ed.), Behavioural models in psychopharmacology: Theoretical, industrial and
clinical perspectilles (pp. 311-330). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Alvir,
J.M.J., Lieberman, J.A., Safferman, A.Z., Schwimmer, J.L., and Schaaf, J.A.
(1993). Clozapine-induced agranulocytosis:
Incidence and risk factors in the United States. New England Journal of
Medicine, 329, 162-167.
American
Psychiatric Association. ( 1985 ).
Statement on tardive dyskinesia.
Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 36, 902-903.
American
Psychiatric Association (1992). Tardive
dyskinesia: A task force report of the American
Psychiatric Association. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.
Bitter,
I., Volavka, J., and Scheurer,
J (1991). The concept of the neuroleptic
threshold: An update. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, II, 28-33.
Bourgeois,
J.A., Drexler, K.G., and Hall M.J. (1991). Hypersalivation
and clozapine [letter to editor]. Hospital
and Community Psychiatry, 42, 1174.
Boyle,
M. (1990). Schizophrenia: A scientific delusion? London
and New York: Routledge.
Breggin,
P.R. (1983). Psychiatric drugs: Hazards to the brain. New York: Springer.
Breggin,
P.R. (1991). Toxic psychiatry: Why therapy, empathy, and love must replace the
drugs, electroshock, and biochemical theories of the "new"
psychiatry. New York: St. Martin's.
Brenner, H.D., Dencker, S.J., Goldstein, M.J., Hubbard, J.W., Keegan,
D.L., Kruger, G., Kulhanek, F., Liberman,
R.B., Maim, U., and Midha, K.K. (
1990). Defining treatment refractoriness in
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 16, 551-562.
Carson, R.C. (1991). Tunnel vision and schizophrenia. 1n W.F. Flack, D.R. Miller,
and M. Wiener (Eds.), What is schizophrenia? (pp.
245-250). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Ciompi,
L., Dauwalder, H.P., Maier, C., Aebi,
E., Triitsch, K., Kupper,
Z., and Rutishauser, C. (1992). The
pilot project "Soteria Berne": Clinical
experiences and results. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161 (supplement),
145-153.
Cohen,
B.M., Keck, P.E., Satlin, A., and Cole, j .0. ( 1991 ). Prevalence and severity of akathisia
in patients on clozapine. Biological Psychiatry, 29,
1215-1219.
Cohen, D. (1988). Social work and psychotropic drug treatments. Social Service
Review, 62, 576-599.
Cohen, D., and McCubbin, M. ( 1990). The
political economy of tardive dyskinesia:
Asymmetries in power and responsibility. Journal of Mind and Behavior
, II, 455-474.
Collins,
E.J., Hogan, T.P., and Awad, A.G. (1992). The
pharmacoepidemiology of treatment- refractory
schizophrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 37, 192-195.
Das Gupta,
K., and Young, A. (1991). Clozapine-induced
neuroleptic malignant syndrome. Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry , 52, 105-107.
Davis,
J.M., Kane, J.M., Marder, S.R., Brauzer,
B., Gierl, B., Schooler, N., Casey, D.E., and Hassan,
M. (1993). Dose response of
prophylactic antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 54 (supplement), 24-30.
DeLeon,
J., Moral, L., and Camuilas, C. (1991).
Clozapine and jaw dyskinesia: A case report. Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry, 52, 494-495.
Denber,
H.C.B. (1959). Side effects of phenothiazines.
In N.S. Kline (Ed.), Psychopharmacology frontiers (pp.
61-62). Boston: Little, Brown and Company.
Deniker, P. (1986). Are the
anti-psychotic drugs to be withdrawn? In C. Shagass, R. Josiassen, and W.
Bridger (Eds.), Biological psychiatry (pp. 1-9). New York: Elsevier.
Deniker, P. (1989). From chlorpromazine to tardive dyskinesia (brief history of the neuroleptics).
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 14, 253-259.
Dixon,
L., Weiden, P.j., Frances, A.j., and Rapkin, B. (1989).
Management of neuroleptic- induced movement disorders:
Effects of physician training. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 104-106.
Doepp,
S., and BudJeberg, C. (1975). Extrapyramidale symptome unter Clozapin. Nervenarzt, 46,
589-590.
Drug
treatment of schizophrenic patients still a quagmire for clinicians. ( 1992, January 3 ). Psychiatric News, 27,
6.
Effect
of risperidone treatment on hospitalization status in
patients with chronic schizophrenia. (1993, March 19). Research report prepared by Janssen Canada.
Easton,
K., and Link, I. (1986--87). Do neuroleptics
prevent relapse? Clinical observations in a psychosocial
rehabilitation program. Psychiatry Quarterly, 58, 42-50.
Fischer, S., and Greenberg,
R.P. ( 1989). The limits of biological treatments for
psychological distress: Comparisons with psychotherapy and placebo. Hillsdale,
New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Fischer,
S., and Greenberg, R.P. (1993). How sound is the
double-blind design for evaluating psychotropic drugs? Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 181, 345-350.
Friedman, J.H. (1993). Akathisia with clozapine [letter to editor]. Biological Psychiatry,
33, 852-853.
Goleman, D.
(1993, August 17). Placebo effect is shown to be twice as powerful as expected.
The New York 1Imes, p. B3.
Grabowksi, J.
(1992). Clonidine treatment of clozapine-induced hypersalivation [letter to editor]. Journal of
Clinical Psychopharmacology, 12, 69-70.
Greenberg, R.P., Bornstein,
R.F., Greenberg, M.D. and Fischer, S. (1992). A meta-analysis
of antidepressant outcome under "blinder" conditions. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 664-669.
Gualtieri,
C.T. (1993). The problem of tardive
akathisia. Brain and Cognition, 23, 102-109.
Healy, D. (1993). Psychopharmacology and the ethics of
resource allocation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 23-29.
Hogarty,
G.E. (1993). Prevention of relapse in chronic schizophrenic
patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54
(supplement), 18-23.
Hollister, L.E. (1994). New psychotherapeutic drugs. Journal of Clinical
Psychopharmacology, 14, 50-63.
Johns, C.E., Mayerhoff, D.I., Lieberman, j.A., and Kane, j.M.
(1990). Schizophrenia: Alternative neuroleptic
strategies. In B. Angrist and S.C. Schulz (Eds.), The neuroleptic-nonresponsive
patient: Characterization and treatment (pp. 51-66). Washington,
D.C.; American Psychiatric Press.
Kane, J.M. (1989). The current status of neuroleptic
therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 50, 322-328.
Kane,
J.M., and Lieberman, J.A. (Eds.). (1992).
Adverse effects of psychotropic drugs. New York: Guilford.
Karon, B. (1989).
Psychotherapy versus medication for schizophrenia: Empirical comparisons. In S.
Fischer and R.P. Greenberg (Eds.), The limits of
biological treatments for psychological distress: Comparisons with
psychotherapy and placebo (pp. 105-150). Hillsdale, New Jersey;
Erlbaum.
Keck,
P.E. jr., Cohen, B.M., Baldessarini,
R., and McElroy, S.L. (1989). Time
course of antipsychotic effects of neuroleptic drugs.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 1289-1292.
Kerwin, R.
W. (1994). The new atypical antipsychotics; A lack of extrapyramidal side-effects and new routes in schizophrenia
research. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 141-148.
Keshavan,
M.S., and Kennedy, J.S. (Eds.). (1992).
Drug-induced dysfunction in psychiatry. New York: Hemisphere.
Kiesler,
C.A., and Sibulkin, A.E. ( 1987).
Mental hositalization: Myths and facts about a
notional crisis. Newbury Park, California; Sage.
Kinon,
C.J., Kane, J.M., johns, C., Perovich, R., Ismi, M., Koreen, A., and Weiden, P. (1993). Treatment
of neuroleptic-resistant schizophrenic relapse.
Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 29, 309-314.
Kuhn,
T. (1970). The structure of
scientific revolutions (second edition) Chicago; University of Chicago Press.
Major advance in
schizophrenia treatment just introduced. (1993, May 12). Press
release. Toronto: Janssen Pharmaceutica Inc.
McEvoy, J.P. (1986). The neuroleptic threshold as a marker of
minimum effective neuroleptic dose.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27, 327-335.
Meltzer, H.Y. (1992). Treatment of the neuroleptic-nonresponsive
schizophrenic patient. Schizophrenia Bulletin,
18,515-542.
Miller,
D.D., Sharafuddin, M.j.A.,
and Kathol, R.G. (1991). A case of clozapine-induced neuroleptic malignant syndrome. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 52, 99-101.
Mitchell, P. (1993).
Chlorpromazine turns forty. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 29, 341-344.
Mosher,
L.R., and Menn, A.Z. (1978).
Community residential treatment for schizophrenia:
Two-year
follow-up. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 29,
715-723.
Nemecek,
D., Rastogi-Cruz, D., and Csernansky,
J.G. (1993). Atropinism may
precipitate neuroleptic malignant syndrome during
treatment with clozapine. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1561.
Nobecourt,
P., and Turgeon, M.-H. (1992, October).
Neuroleptiques classiques et neuroleptiques atypiques [Classical and atypical neuroleptics).
Le Medecin du Quebec, 27, 67-73.
Paul,
G.L., and Lentz, R.J. (1977). Psychosocial treatment of
chronic mental patients: Milieu versus social leaming
approaches. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Payer,
L. (1990). Medicine and culture: Notions of health and
sickness in Britain, the United States, France and West Germany. London: Victor
Gollancz Ltd.
Reardon,
G.T., Rifkin, A., Schwartz, A., Myerson, A., and Siris,
S.G. (1989). Changing patterns of neuroleptic dosage over a decade. American Journal
of Psychiatry, 146, 726-729.
Reddig,
S., Minnema, A.M., and Tandon,
R. (1993). Neuroleptic malignant syndrome and clozapine.
Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 5, 25-27.
Rifkin, A. ( 1987). Exttapyramidal
side-effects: A historical perspective. Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry, 48 (supplement), 3-6.
Rifkin,
A., Doddi, S., Karajgi, B.,
Borenstein, M., and Wachspress,
M. (1991). Dosage of haloperidol for
schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 166-170.
Rosen,
S.I., and Hanno, P.M. (1992). Clozapine-induced
priapism. Journal of Urology, 148, 876-877.
Sarbin,T. (1990). Toward the
obsolescence of the schizophrenia hypothesis. Journal of Mind and
Behavior, II, 259-283.
Schwartz,
J.T., and Brotman, A.W. (1992). A clinical guide to antipsychotic drugs. Drugs, 44, 981-992.
Seftel,
A.D., Saenz de Tejada, I., Szetela,
B., Cole, J., and Goldstein, I. ( 1992).
Clozapine-associated priapism: A case report. Journal
of Urology, 147, 146-148.
Segal,
S.P., Cohen, D., and Marder, S.R. (1992). Neuroleptic medication and prescription pat- terns with
sheltered-care residents: A 12-year perspective. American Journal of Public
Health, 88, 846-852.
Stoll, A.L, Tohen, M., Baldessarini, R.J.,
Goodwin, D.C., Stein, S., Katz, S., Geenens, D., Swinson, R.P., Goethe, J.W., and McGlashan,
T. (1993). Shifts in diagnostic frequencies of schizophrenia
and major affective disorders at six North American psychiatric hospitals,
1972-1988. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1668-1673.
Szymanski,
S., Jody, D., Leipzig, R., Masiar, S., and Lieberman,
J. (1991). Anticholinergic
delirium caused by retreatment with clozapine [letter
to editor). American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1752.
Task
Force on Late Neurological Effects of Antipsychotic Drugs. (1980).
Tardive dyskinesia: Summary
of a task force report of the American Psychiatric Association. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 137,1163-1172.
Turns,
C.N. (1990). Effects of sedatives and neuroleptics
[letter to editor). American Journal of Psychiatry, 147,
1576.
Van Putten,
T., and Marder, S.R. ( 1987).
Behavioral toxicity of antipsychotic drugs. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48 (supplement), 13-19.
Waddington,
J.L., Weller, M.P., Crow, T.J., and Hirsch, S.R. (1992).
Schizophrenia, genetic retrenchment, and epidemiologic renaissance: The Sixth
Biennial Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Badgastein,
Austria, January 26-February 1, 1992. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 990-994.
Warner, R. ( 1985 ). Recovery from schizophrenia: Psychiatry and
political economy. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Weiden,
P.J., Mann, J.J., Haas, G., Mattson, M., and Frances, A. (1987).
Clinical nonrecogni- tion
of neuroleptic-induced movement disorders: A
cautionary study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1148-1153.
WeIler,
M., and Korhuber, J. (1993).
Does clozapine cause neuroleptic
malignant syndrome? [letter to editor] Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry , 54, 70-71.
White. K.,
Kando. J., Patk. T.. Watemaux, C., and Brown. W.A. (1992). Side effects and the "blindability" of clinical drug trials. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1730-1731.
Wiebe,
E.J. (1993). Weight gain with clozapine
treatment [letter to editor]. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 70.
Wiener,
M. (1991). Schizophrenia: A defective, Deficient,
disrupted, disorganized construct. In W.F. Flack, D.R.
Miller. and M. Wiener (Eds.), What is
schizophrenia? (pp. 199-222). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Windgassen, K.
(1992). Treatment with neuroleptics: The patients. perspective. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 86,
405-410.
Wysowsky. D.K., and Baum. C. ( 1989). Antipsychotic drug use in the United States. 1976-1985. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46,929-932.
Ziegler. J.,
and Behar, D. (1992). Clozapine-induced priapism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149,
272-273.
1, According to Kerwin (1994), all risperidone
trials involved a short washout period of one week. By itself, this may easily
explain unusual findings such as extrapyramidal effects
in the "placebo" group. That such a predictable confound is ignored
and results elevated to the sra- tus
of a major improvement in schizophrenia treatment shows the desperation in the
drug treatment field (see below).
2, For
different views on the nature of "schizophrenia," see Boyle (1990),
Sarbin (1990), and Wiener (1991), among others. Stall, Tohen,
Baldessarini, Goodwin, Stein et al. (1993) reported a
300% decrease in the frequency of schizophrenia diagnoses (
in parallel with a 400% increase in the frequency of diagnoses of major
affective disorders) in six North American psychiatric teaching hospitals from
1972 to 1988. These results highlight the absurdity of viewing these conditions
as genetically provoked brain diseases.
3, lt
is beyond the scope of this paper to discuss the inappropriateness of the term
"side effect." Although we find in the psychopharmacological
literature a clear distinction between "pri- mary" and "secondary," or "main"
and "side" effects, there are few discussions of what a
"side" effect is. What distinguishes the two types of effects appears
to be simply the intent of the pre- scriber and has nothing to do with the
pharmacological action of the drug, nor does it rest on any
"objective" consideration such as frequency, intensity, or Juration of the effect.
4, It
is unusual to see such high estimates of EPS and such frank statements to the
effect that EPS cause relapse, especially from a pharmaceutical company.
However, risperidone is mat- keted
on the basis of its low propensiry to produce EPS:
statements that other neuroleptics produce a high
rate of EPS thus reflect favorably on the new drug. This tendency, to frankly
and publicly acknowledge the ill-effects of widely used treatments only when a
new treatment arrives on the scene, characterizes the introduction of most
somatic treatments in psychiatry (insulin coma, ECT, lobotomy, neuroleptics, and now, "atypical" neuroleptics}.
5, According to the neuroleptic threshold theory, the "threshold
dose" (or minimum effective antipsychotic dose of a neuroleptic)
correlates with the appearance of "fine motor" symptoms (micrography) as opposed to the appearance of manifest or
"coarse motor" EPS (see Bitter et al., 1991; McEvoy, 1986).
6, The
risk of agranulocytosis, however, has been
well-publicized: between February 1990 and April 1991, 73 of 11,555 patients on
clozapine developed this complication, and two died (Alvir, Lieberman, Safferman, Schwimmer, and Schaaf, 1993).
7, The well-known fact that
daily neuroleptic doses are, on average, much higher
in the United States than in most European countries raises a host of other
interesting questions about the social construction of the efficacy of
treatment (see Payer, 1990).